Добавил:
Upload Опубликованный материал нарушает ваши авторские права? Сообщите нам.
Вуз: Предмет: Файл:
[Jan_Scott]_Cognitive_Therapy_in_Clinical_Practice(BookSee.org).pdf
Скачиваний:
15
Добавлен:
08.03.2016
Размер:
1.6 Mб
Скачать

96 COGNITIVE THERAPY IN CLINICAL PRACTICE

to introspect and give an account of the circumstances of their last relapse. Others will find this more difficult and will need to practise self-monitoring before the factors emerge. For those few people who are unable to offer any cues it may be necessary to interview friends or family members to obtain the relevant information (see Table 7.3).

Problem solving and cue modification

The distinction between general problem-solving strategies as used in cognitive therapy with other disorders and interventions to prevent relapse is an artificial one. In practice many of the problems in the addict’s life link in with the risk of relapse in some way. We will therefore concentrate on how the common problems associated with internal and external trigger factors can be dealt with. The aims of cognitive therapy in this context are twofold: first, to teach the person effective problemsolving skills and coping strategies to deal with addiction, and second, to alter, wherever possible, the underlying predisposition to use drugs or engage in other maladaptive behaviour.

Modifying situational factors

Cue exposure and aversion

Situational factors come up frequently as cues for drug taking. The ideal course of action would be to make individuals invulnerable to the effects of visiting places where they have used drugs before, been with other addicts, seen people intoxicated, etc. This is rarely possible, and so strategies of avoidance and coping with craving need to be taught. There are, however, some behavioural methods which can be used to weaken the power that situational factors exert over the individual. These were initially developed out of classical conditioning theories of addiction, and involve variations of either aversion or exposure. Aversion therapy, which used punishments such as electric shocks or emetics, is less fashionable than in the past, and there is evidence to suggest it is less effective than cognitive-behavioural methods (Marlatt and George 1983). One form of aversion therapy, which uses imagery, may be suitable for selected patients. Cautela’s covert sensitisation (Cautela 1966, 1967) requires the

Table 7.4 Hierarchy of stimuli producing craving

Stimulus

Estimated craving %

 

 

Picture of doctor’s surgery

50

Tourniquet and spoon

60

Empty syringe

65

Syringe and needle

70

Syringe and needle containing physeptone

80

Drawing up drug from an ampoule

85

Sitting with syringe and needle against arm

90

patient to visualise in detail a scene where the drug is being consumed, making sure that the cues which have been identified are part of the picture. During this imagery the therapist checks regularly that there is an affective response to the cognitive manipulation, i.e. anticipation and craving. At the point of injection, inhalation, or ingestion, when the addict would experience the effect of the drug, they are instructed to switch the image to an unpleasant one. One of the keys to the successful use of this technique seems to be selecting the most appropriately aversive image. Unpleasant physical experiences such vomiting are often effective, but for others social punishments might be aversive and therefore have more effect, e.g. being discovered intoxicated by your mother. This pairing of images is repeated several times within the session and then the addict practises it several times a day.

In Jane’s case we chose to use a form of cue exposure rather than covert conditioning. Her sensitivity to the ritualistic aspects of her drug use made it suitable to use an in vivo procedure where the stimuli were the materials of drug taking. Table 7.4 is the hierarchy of stimuli arranged in order of increasing importance. It can be seen that the more closely the situation approximated to actually taking the drug the greater the expected craving. Some of the data from the exposure sessions are presented in Figure 7.1. Within each session there was a reduction in both craving and anxiety over time. Across sessions the level of craving at the beginning of each session reduced, while the time taken for the craving to reduce became progressively shorter. This patient was one of the subjects for a pilot study of cue exposure with addicts (Bradley and Moorey 1987). The method and results presented so far are no different from any purely behavioural exposure task, but the technique was used as part of a cognitive-behavioural programme. There were a number of ways in which this intervention was

DRUG ABUSERS 97

Figure 7.1 Change in craving during exposure to stimulus (tourniquet)

cognitive as well as behavioural. First, the exposure was presented as a behavioural experiment in which the subject tested out her prior belief that in the presence of the stimulus the craving would not reduce. Emphasis was placed on the way that Jane was actively engaged in the process of coping with her craving. The whole exercise became a form of graded task assignment to increase her self-efficacy. Second, at 5-minute intervals Jane recorded her automatic thoughts and images, reinforcing the relevance of cognitive factors in her understanding of the problem. Third, the exercise was used to teach Jane two cognitive strategies, distraction and challenging cognitions, which she could use to cope if exposed to difficult situations in the future.

Thought monitoring during exposure The exposure task provided the therapist with valuable information about the cognitions which Jane experienced when she was craving. These thoughts seemed to be grouped into four main categories which are listed with examples:

Pleasant aspects of drug use

I’d enjoy a fix.

The heroin looks familiar and comforting.

Unpleasant aspects of drug use

I’m not even sure I would enjoy how this drug would make me feel.

Having a fix of heroin would be a hassle.

Intent to use the drug

I’d like to risk taking this linctus.

I want a fix.

Intent not to use the drug

I’m sure I wouldn’t fix this heroin.

I can’t wait to throw this heroin away.

As might be expected the cognitions which were more positive towards drug abuse were reported at the beginning of the sessions when craving was high. As craving reduced, the cognitions focused more on the negative aspects of the experience or the wish to give up drugs. This suggests a correlation between craving and cognitions and raises interesting questions about which changes first in the exposure, thoughts or urges. There may well be an interaction between cognitions and craving such as is found in other feeling states like anxiety and depression. This is a fruitful area for research which has, as yet, hardly begun to be explored. The experience of Jane and other addicts with whom this method has been tried is that over time they experience less and less craving in the face of the stimulus, and develop a sense of mastery over their craving. This method may be associated with high levels of craving at the beginning and it should be used with caution if a patient is already experiencing high craving.

Predicting and avoiding high-risk situations

Situational factors are often so powerful that it is a wise strategy to avoid them unless absolutely necessary. Most addicts are only too aware of this, but some have very rigid and absolute beliefs about the strength of volition, e.g. ‘I have to test myself out’, ‘If I’m strong enough to give up I will be strong enough to face any temptation’. Cognitive techniques can be used to challenge the validity and usefulness of these beliefs. The therapist can ask patients to look at the advantages and