Добавил:
Опубликованный материал нарушает ваши авторские права? Сообщите нам.
Вуз: Предмет: Файл:
The Nature of Culture.pdf
Скачиваний:
29
Добавлен:
11.11.2021
Размер:
6.21 Mб
Скачать

10 Stone Tools

115

(Stiner and Kuhn 1992). Examination of the technology suggests that different local solutions were found to foraging options within what generally would be classied as Mousterian industries (Kuhn 2006). Although often the same prey were killed, often at prime age, in Middle and in Upper Paleolithic contexts, the methods by which they were taken seem likely to have differed, with greater reliance on technology to increase reliable resource production in the Upper Paleolithic, probably increasing rates of child survivorship and hence population density (Stiner 2006).

In brief, the Late Pleistocene record allows analysis of the archaeological evidence freed from the worst problems of taphonomy and dating. As a result, the resolution of the record allows faunal remains and technology to be interpreted in terms of the plausible foraging strategies and consequences of hominins at different times and allows comparisons between hominins. The patterning is not well represented by the coarse taxonomic units derived from the nineteenth century classications. In each region populations adapted to local circumstances and did not necessarily reach solutions that look the same within a simplistic analytical framework: they had what Kuhn called rugged tness landscapes with many local and regional tness peaks (Kuhn 2006). It may be that such ne-grained analysis is not possible for the earlier record, but these case studies show that, in addition to the other problems of interpretation outlined here, the analysis of stone artifact typology through time at one site, or by assigning assemblages to one or other class which is then interpreted in terms of the behavior of culturesdened by the existence of those classes, can miss the whole point that the tools were made for use and were part of the behavioral repertoire of hominins or people.

Binford, following White (1959:8), always claimed that culture was an extrasomatic means of adaptation (Binford 1962), but he did not mean that once the regularities in the archaeological remains have been given a name that is mistakenly called a culture,the adaptation can be understood. Rather, the adaptation can be seen in the way in which patterning in the artifacts was reproduced from time to time and place to place, and used in consistent ways at one time and different ways at another time, all of which inuenced the success of particular foraging strategies and impacted on the demographic success of the population. The culture is found in the way in which artifacts were used, not only in their form. The technology of stone tools was in the knowledge associated with (a) procurement of tool stone,

(b) aking the stone, (c) using the tool to make another tool, perhaps of wood or bone or skin, (d) using both in the acquisition of resources, (e) the knowledge of the resources that were made accessible through of the use of those tools, and (f) in preparing and consuming the resources once acquired, not just in the form of the product. It was technology that was socially learned, not just the form of the

tool. And the cultural knowledge involved both the technology so-dened as well as the social congurations of the hominins or people who used it.

Conclusion

Primatologists and archaeologists share the problem that their subjects cannot speak to them about what they are doing. By adopting some of the methodology by which primatologists have determined whether social learning occurs among them, it has been possible to show up some of the deciencies in the existing arguments about the culturalnature of early stone industries. Most importantly, it is not clear that archaeologists necessarily even understand what it was that was learned when knapping skills were passed from one generation to another. I tentatively suggest that for the Oldowan it may have been no more than the ability to remove a succession of akes from a platform: the knappers had the cognitive abilities to exploit angles when encountered but not to create new ones(Delagnes and Roche 2005). For the Acheulean it may have been about the maximisation through core rotations of the number of akes struck from the acute platform created symmetrically at the margins of a large ake. The Levallois technique enabled the repeated production of large numbers of akes from asymmetric cores by the discovery that as core angles tended towards obtuseness those angles could be renewed by the removal of a large ake. The Mousterian represented the recognition that the use-life of akes could be prolonged by repeated retouching and reshaping.

Using the criteria about the consistency of behavior within a group, the variation between groups and the persistence of a tradition through time, it is possible to highlight the weaknesses of arguments about the cultural nature of knapping behavior and set up a number of research questions that need to be investigated. For each major grouping, is it possible to determine the extent of consistency, variation and persistence of tradition in this way? What is the incidence of knapping errors at particular times and places in relation to different raw material availability that might indicate what was being learned and how?

Several studies suggest that, in addition, the way ahead requires an assessment of the organisation of behavior in relation to stone tool production and use, especially in the context of other behavior, such as food acquisition. The outcomes may well not produce simple unilinear progress towards modernity or the Upper Paleolithic, and, indeed, may produce different adaptive solutions which made such progress unlikely. Such subtleties will produce a way of looking at adaptation very different from the view derived from nineteenth century archaeology. At its best, such an

116

I. Davidson

approach may permit archaeologists to look at the impact of technology (tools as well as their role in society) on population size, and planning depth. One of the ways in which technology had such impacts was through the ability of hominins to expand either by nding a niche that was common to several habitats, or by nding new niches within a single habitat, or ultimately nding new niches that enabled them to move into new habitats.

The question of what labels should be given to such sorts of culture is complicated by the argument in this paper that much of the standard description of variation, in terms of artifact types, is not recognizing the appropriate variations. In Tomasellos (1999:5) words, cumulative culture, the distinctive property of modern humans that is absent in apes, requires not only creative invention but also, and just as importantly, faithful social transmission that can work as a ratchet to prevent slippage backward so that the newly invented artifact or practice preserves its new and improved form at least somewhat faithfully until a further modication or improvement comes along.

If it is true that conventional accounts of stone artifact variation do not capture what are the creative inventions, it is difcult to see how archaeology can study whether cumulative culture was present or not. The simplied set of changes outlined in this paper may offer some chance of asking the right questions. One other possibility is represented by the possibility of modular learning. I indicated, above, the suggestion that chunkingof sequences of removals permitted expert learningof the Levallois technique (Wynn and Coolidge 2010). There is an extent to which this conclusion is dependent on the belief in the standard interpretation of the Levallois technique, which seems implausible. I have previously suggested that a better candidate for such modular learning is the use of crested blades as a preparation for the production of blade cores in the Middle Stone Age of southern Africa and the European Upper Paleolithic (Davidson 2010b:197198). In those cases, the cognitive signicance came from the capacity to engage in a physical activity that appears unrelated to the nal purpose of it (see discussion, also, in Haidle 2011:460461). This suggestion could also lead to future research to establish whether there were similar modular knapping sequences in other industries.

In conclusion, the standard way of looking at stone artifacts has not yielded insights that are helpful in a study of social learning in hominin evolution but there are ways of looking at knapping that probably allow a way forward.

Acknowledgments I thank Miriam Haidle and Nick Conard for the invitation to the conference and all the conference participants for their friendly discussion and disagreements. Among those from whom I have received important guidance on issues related to this paper are Phil Barnard, Miriam Belmaker, Rob Gargett, Barbara King, Bill McGrew, Mark Moore, Kimberley Newman, April Nowell, and Pat Shipman. Kimberley Newman prepared the nal version of the gures. I thank them all.

References

Arnold, M. (1869). Culture and anarchy: An essay in social and political criticism. Retrieved April 4, 2012 from http://www. gutenberg.org/catalog/world/readle?fk_les=1724531&pageno=1 Baena, J., Lordkipanidze, D., Cuartero, F., Ferring, R., Zhvania, D., Martín, D., et al. (2010). Technical and technological complexity in the beginning: The study of Dmanisi lithic assemblage. Quaternary

International, 223224, 4553.

Bar-Yosef, O., & Van Peer, P. (2009). The Chaîne Opératoire approach in Middle Paleolithic archaeology. Current Anthropology, 50, 103131.

Barnard, P. J. (2010). Current developments in inferring cognitive capabilities from the archaeological traces left by stone tools: Caught between a rock and a hard inference. In A. Nowell & I. Davidson (Eds.), Stone tools and the evolution of human cognition

(pp. 207226). Boulder, CO: University Press of Colorado. Barnard, P. J., Duke, D. J., Byrne, R. W., & Davidson, I. (2007).

Differentiation in cognitive and emotional meanings: An evolutionary analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 11551183.

Bennett, T. (2005). Culture. In T. Bennett, L. Grossberg, & M. Morris (Eds.), New keywords. A revised vocabulary of culture and society

(pp. 6369). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.

Beyries, S. (1987). Variabilité de lindustrie lithique au Moustérien. Oxford: British Archaeological Reports International Series 328.

Binford, L. R. (1962). Archaeology as anthropology. American Antiquity, 28, 217225.

Binford, L. R. (1972). Contemporary model building: Paradigms and the current state of Palaeolithic research. In D. L. Clarke (Ed.), Models in Archaeology (pp. 109166). London: Methuen.

Binford, L. R. (1980). Willow smoke and dogstails: Hunter-gatherer settlement systems and archaeological site formation. American Antiquity, 45, 420.

Binford, L. R., & Binford, S. R. (1966). A preliminary analysis of functional variability in the Mousterian of Levallois facies.

American Anthropologist, 68, 238295.

Binford, L. R., & OConnell, J. F. (1984). An Alyawara day: The stone quarry. Journal of Anthropological Research, 40, 406432.

Boëda, E. (1988). Le concept Levallois et evaluation de son champ dapplication. In M. Otte (Ed.), LHomme de Néandertal, Vol. 4: La Technique (pp. 1326). ERAUL 31, Liège: Univertsité de Liège.

Boesch, C. (1991). Teaching among wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 41, 530532.

Boesch, C., & Boesch, H. (1984a). Mental map in wild chimpanzees: An analysis of hammer transports for nut cracking. Primates, 25, 160170.

Boesch, C., & Boesch, H. (1984b). Possible causes of sex differences in the use of natural hammers by wild chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 13, 415440.

Boesch, C., & Boesch, H. (1993). Aspects of transmission of tool-use in wild chimpanzees. In K. Gibson & T. Ingold (Eds.), Tools, language and cognition in human evolution (pp. 171183). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Boesch, C., Marchesi, P., Marchesi, N., Fruth, B., & Joulian, F. (1994). Is nut cracking in wild chimpanzees a cultural behaviour? Journal of Human Evolution, 26, 325338.

Bonner, J. (1980). The evolution of culture in animals. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Bordes, F. (1961). Typologie du Paléolithique Ancien et Moyen. Bordeaux: Delmas.

Bordes, F., & de Sonneville-Bordes, D. (1970). The signicance of varibility in Palaeolithic assemblages. World Archaeology, 2, 6173.

Bowler, J. M., Jones, R., Allen, H., & Thorne, A. G. (1970). Pleistocene human remains from Australia: A living site and human

10 Stone Tools

117

cremation from Lake Mungo, Western New South Wales. World Archaeology, 2, 3960.

Box, H. O. & Gibson, K. R. (1999). Preface. In H. O. Box & K. R. Gibson (Eds.), Mammalian social learning. Comparative and ecological perspectives (pp. xxiv). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Bradley, B., & Sampson, C. G. (1986). Analysis by replication of two Acheulian artefact assemblages. In G. N. Bailey & P. Callow (Eds.), Stone age prehistory (pp. 2945). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Brown, P. (2002). Language as a model for culture: Lessons from the cognitive sciences. In R. G. Fox & B. J. King (Eds.), Anthropology beyond culture (pp. 169192). Oxford: Berg.

Brumm, A., & McLaren, A. (2011). Scraper reduction and imposed format the Lower Palaeolithic site of High Lodge, England.

Journal of Human Evolution, 60, 185204.

Clark, G. A. & Riel-Salvatore, J. (2006). Observations on systematics in Paleolithic archaeology. In E. Hovers & S. Kuhn (Eds.),

Transitions before the transition. Evolution and stability in the Middle Paleolithic and the Middle Stone Age (pp. 2956). New York: Springer.

Collard, M., & Wood, B. (2000). How reliable are human phylogenetic hypotheses? Proceedings of the National Academy of the Sciences, 97, 50035006.

Coolidge, F. L., & Wynn, T. (2009). The rise of Homo sapiens. The evolution of modern thinking. Oxford: Blackwell.

Davidson, I. (1999). The game of the name: Continuity and discontinuity in language origins. In B. J. King (Ed.), The origins of language. What nonhuman primates can tell us (pp. 229268). Santa Fe, NM: School of American Research.

Davidson, I. (2002). The Finished Artefact Fallacy: Acheulean handaxes and language origins. In A. Wray (Ed.), Transitions to language (pp. 180203). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Davidson, I. (2003). The archaeological evidence of language origins: States of art. In M. H. Christiansen & S. Kirby (Eds.), Language evolution (pp. 140157). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Davidson, I. (2009). Comment on Bar-Yosef and Van Peer. Current Anthropology, 50, 119120.

Davidson, I. (2010a). The archaeology of cognitive evolution. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 214229.

Davidson, I. (2010b). Stone tools and the evolution of hominin and human cognition. In A. Nowell & I. Davidson (Eds.), Stone tools and the evolution of human cognition (pp. 185205). Boulder, CO: University Press of Colorado.

Davidson, I. (2012). Origins of pictures: An argument for transformation of signs. In K. Sachs-Hombach & J. R. J. Schirra (Eds.),

Origins of Pictures. Anthropological Discourses in Image Science. Cologne: Halem.

Davidson, I. (2014). Its the thought that counts: Unpacking the package of behaviour of the rst people of Australia and its adjacent Islands. In R. Dennell & M. Porr (Eds.), Southern Asia, Australia and the search for human origins (pp. 243256). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Davidson, I., & McGrew, W. C. (2005). Stone tools and the uniqueness of human culture. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 11, 793817.

Davidson, I., & Noble, W. (1993). Tools and language in human evolution. In K. Gibson & T. Ingold (Eds.), Tools, language and cognition in human evolution (pp. 363388). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

de Sonneville-Bordes, D. (1986). Technique Levallois dans l'Australie du Sud-Est. Arqueologia (Porto), 13, 7175.

de la Torre, I. (2010). Insights on the technical competence of the early Oldowan. In A. Nowell & I. Davidson (Eds.), Stone tools and the evolution of human cognition (pp. 4565). Boulder, CO: University Press of Colorado.

de la Torre, I., Mora, R., Domínguez-Rodrigo, M., de Luque, L., & Alcalá, L. (2003). The Oldowan industry of Peninj and its bearing on the reconstruction of the technological skills of Lower Pleistocene hominids. Journal of Human Evolution, 44, 203224.

Deetz, J. (1967). Invitation to Archaeology. New York: Natural History Press.

Delagnes, A. & Meignen, L. (2006). Diversity of lithic production systems during the Middle Paleolithic in France. Are there any chronological trends? In E. Hovers & S. Kuhn (Eds.), Transitions before the transition. Evolution and stability in the Middle Paleolithic and the Middle Stone Age (pp. 85107). New York: Springer.

Delagnes, A., & Roche, H. (2005). Late Pliocene hominid knapping skills: The case of Lokalalei 2C, West Turkana, Kenya. Journal of Human Evolution, 48, 435472.

Dennell, R. W., Martinón-Torres, M., & Bermúdez de Castro, J. M. (2011). Hominin variability, climatic instability and population demography in Middle Pleistocene Europe. Quaternary Science Reviews, 30, 15111524.

Dibble, H. L. (1984). Interpreting typological variation of Middle Paleolithic scrapers: Function, style, or sequence of reduction?

Journal of Field Archaeology, 11, 431436.

Dibble, H. L. (1987). Reduction sequences in the manufacture of Mousterian implements of France. In O. Soffer (Ed.), The Pleistocene old world: Regional perspectives (pp. 3344). New York: Plenum Press.

Dibble, H. L. (1989). The implications of stone tool types for the presence of language during the Lower and Middle Paleolithic. In P. A. Mellars & C. B. Stringer (Eds.), The human revolution: Behavioural and biological perspectives on the origins of modern humans (pp. 415431). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Dominguez-Rodrigo, M., Serrallonga, J., Juan-Tresserras, J., Alcala, L., & Luque, L. (2001). Woodworking activities by early humans: A plant residue analysis on Acheulian stone tools from Peninj (Tanzania). Journal of Human Evolution, 40, 289299.

Enquist, M., & Ghirlanda, S. (2007). Evolution of social learning does not explain the origin of human cumulative culture. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 246, 129135.

Féblot-Augustins, J. (1997). La circulation des matières premières au Paléolithique (Vol. 1 et 2. ERAUL 75). Liège: Université de Liège.

Foley, R., & Lahr, M. M. (1997). Mode 3 technologies and the evolution of modern humans. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 7(1), 336.

Gagneux, P., Gonder, M. K., Goldberg, T. L., & Morin, P. A. (2001). Gene ow in wild chimpanzee populations: What genetic data tell us about chimpanzee movement over space and time. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 356, 889897.

Gibbs, S., Collard, M., & Wood, B. (2000). Soft-tissue characters in higher primate phylogenetics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 97, 1113011132.

Goddard, C. (2005). The lexical semantics of culture. Language Sciences, 27, 5173.

Goldman-Neuman, T. & Hovers, E. (2009). Methodological considerations in the study of Oldowan raw material selectivity: Insights from A.L. 894 (Hadar, Ethiopia). In E. Hovers & D. R. Braun (Eds.), Interdisciplinary approaches to the Oldowan (pp. 7184). New York: Springer.

Goldman-Neuman, T., & Hovers, E. (2012). Raw material selectivity in Late Pliocene Oldowan sites in the Makaamitalu Basin, Hadar, Ethiopia. Journal of Human Evolution, 62, 353366.

Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe. Patterns of behavior. Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

Gowlett, J. A. J. (1984). Mental abilities of early Man: A look at some hard evidence. In R. A. Foley (Ed.), Hominid Evolution and Community Ecology (pp. 167192). London: Academic Press.

118

 

I. Davidson

Green, R. E., Krause, J., Briggs, A. W., Maricic, T., Stenzel, U.,

Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory

Kircher, M., et al. (2010). A draft sequence of the Neandertal

of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity.

genome. Science, 328, 710722.

 

Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 156185.

Haidle, M. N. (2010). Working memory capacity and the evolution of

Keeley, L. H., & Toth, N. (1981). Microwear polishes on early stone

modern cognitive potential: Implications from animal and early

tools from Koobi Fora, Kenya. Nature, 293, 464465.

human tool use. Current Anthropology, 51, S149S166.

Kinani, J. -F., & Zimmerman, D. (2014). Tool use for food acquisition

Haidle, M. N. (2011). Archaeological approaches to cognitive evolu-

in a wild mountain gorilla (Gorilla beringei beringei). American

tion. In D. B. Kronenfeld, G. Bennardo, V. C. de Munck, & M.

Journal of Primatology, doi:10.1002/ajp.22351.

D. Fischer (Eds.), A companion to cognitive

Anthropology

King, B. J. (2002). Patterned interactions and culture in great apes.

(pp. 450467). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.

 

In R. G. Fox & B. J. King (Eds.), Anthropology beyond culture

Haidle, M. N., & Conard, N. J. (2011). The nature of culture. Synthesis

(pp. 83104). Oxford: Berg.

of an interdisciplinary symposium held in Tübingen, Germany, 15

Klein, R. (2005). Hominin dispersals in the Old World. In C. Scarre

18 June 2011. Mitteilungen der Gesellschaft für Urgeschichte, 20,

(Ed.), The human past. World prehistory and the development of

6578.

 

human societies (pp. 84123). London: Thames and Hudson.

Hay, R. L. (1976). Geology of Olduvai Gorge. Berkeley: University of

Kroeber, A. L., & Kluckhohn, C. (1963). Culture: A critical review of

California Press.

 

concepts and denitions. New York: Vintage Books.

Hayden, B. (1979). Palaeolithic reections: Lithic technology and

Kuhn, S. L. (2006). Trajectories of change in the Middle Paleolithic.

ethnographic excavations among Australian Aborigines. Canberra:

In E. Hovers & S. Kuhn (Eds.), Transitions before the transition.

Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies.

 

Evolution and stability in the Middle Paleolithic and the Middle

Henrich, J. (2004). Demography and cultural evolution: How adaptive

Stone Age (pp. 109120). New York: Springer.

cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses the Tasmanian

Kuhn, S. L. (2010). On standardization in the Paleolithic: Measures,

case. American Antiquity, 69, 197214.

 

causes, and interpretations of metric similarity in stone tools. In A.

Henshilwood, C. S., & Marean, C. W. (2003). The origin of modern

Nowell & I. Davidson (Eds.), Stone tools and the evolution of

human behavior: Critique of the models and their test implications.

human cognition (pp. 105134). Boulder, CO: University Press of

Current Anthropology, 44, 627651.

 

Colorado.

Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernández-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B., &

Kuper, A. (1999). Culture. The anthropologistsaccount. Cambridge,

Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of

MA: Harvard University Press.

social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317,

Laland, K. N. (2008). Animal cultures. Current Biology, 18, R366

13601366.

 

R370.

Hiscock, P., & Attenbrow, V. (2005). Reduction continuums and tool

Laland, K. N., & Hoppitt, W. (2003). Do animals have culture?

use. In C. Clarkson & L. Lamb (Eds.), Lithics Down Under:

Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 150159.

Australian perspectives on lithic reduction, use and classication

Laland, K. N., & Janik, V. M. (2006). The animal cultures debate.

(pp. 4355). Oxford: British Archaeological Reports, Archaeopress.

Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 21, 542547.

Hohmann, G., & Fruth, B. (2003). Culture

in bonobos?

Langergraber, K. E., Boesch, C., Inoue, E., Inoue-Murayama, M.,

Between-species and within species variation in behavior. Current

Mitani, J. C., Nishida, T., et al. (2011). Genetic and cultural

Anthropology, 44, 563571.

 

similarity in wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B:

Hovers, E. (2009a). Learning from mistakes: Flaking accidents and

Biological Sciences, 278, 408416.

knapping skills in the assemblage of A.L. 894, (Hadar, Ethiopia).

Leakey, M. D. (1971). Olduvai Gorge. Volume 3. Excavations in Beds I

In K. Schick & N. Toth (Eds.), The cutting edge: New approaches

and II, 19601963. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

to the archaeology of human origins (pp. 137150). Gosport,

Lieberman, D. E., & Shea, J. J. (1994). Behavioral differences between

Indiana: Stone Age Institute.

 

archaic and modern humans in the Levantine Mousterian. American

Hovers, E. (2009b). The lithic assemblages of Qafzeh Cave. New York:

Anthropologist, 96, 300332.

Oxford University Press.

 

Lipson, S., & Pilbeam, D. (1982). Ramapithecus and Hominoid

Hrdlicka, A. (1927). The Neanderthal phase of man. The Journal of the

evolution. Journal of Human Evolution, 11, 545548.

Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland, 57,

Locke, J. L., & Bogin, B. (2006). Language and life history. Behavioral

249274.

 

and Brain Sciences, 29, 259325.

Iovita, R., & McPherron, S. P. (2011). The handaxe reloaded: A

Lycett, S. J., & Norton, C. J. (2010). A demographic model for

morphometric reassessment of Acheulian and Middle Paleolithic

Palaeolithic technological evolution: The case of East Asia and the

handaxes. Journal of Human Evolution, 61, 6174.

 

Movius Line. Quaternary International, 211, 5565.

Isaac, G. L. (1972). Early phases of human behaviour: Models in Lower

Lycett, S. J., & von Cramon-Taubadel, N. (2008). Acheulean variability

Palaeolithic archaeology. In D. L. Clarke (Ed.), Models in

and hominin dispersals: A model bound approach. Journal of

Archaeology (pp. 167199). London: Methuen.

 

Archaeological Science, 35, 553562.

Isaac, G. L. (1977). Olorgesailie. Archaeological studies of a Middle

Madden, J. R. (2008). Do bowerbirds exhibit cultures? Animal

Pleistocene lake basin in Kenya. Chicago: The University of

Cognition, 11, 112.

Chicago Press.

 

Marshall, G. D., Gamble, C. G., Roe, D. A., & Dupplaw, D. (2002).

Isaac, G. L. (1978). The food-sharing behavior of protohuman

Acheulian biface database. from ADS, York. http://ads.ahds.ac.uk/

hominids. Scientic American, 238, 90108.

 

catalogue/specColl/bifaces/bf_query.cfm

Janik, V. M., & Slater, P. J. B. (2003). Traditions in mammalian and

Marwick, B. (2003). Pleistocene exchange networks as evidence for the

avian vocal communication. In S. Perry & D. Fragaszy (Eds.), The

evolution of language. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 13, 67

biology of tradition: Models and evidence (pp. 213235). Cam-

81.

bridge: Cambridge University Press.

 

McGrew, W. C. (1992). Chimpanzee material culture. Implications for

Kamilar, J. M., & Marshack, J. L. (2012). Does geography or ecology

human evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

best explain culturalvariation among chimpanzee communities?

McGrew, W. C. (2004). The cultured chimpanzee: Reections on

Journal of Human Evolution, 62, 256260.

 

cultural primatology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

10 Stone Tools

119

McGrew, W. C., & Tutin, C. E. G. (1978). Evidence for social custom in wild chimpanzees? Man, 13, 234251.

McPherron, S. P. (2000). Handaxes as a measure of the mental capabilities of early hominids. Journal of Archaeological Science, 27, 655663.

Mellars, P. A. (1969). The chronology of Mousterian industries in the Périgord region of South-West France. Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society, 35, 134171.

Mellars, P. A. (1989). Technological changes across the Middle-Upper Palaeolithic transition: Economic, social and cognitive perspectives. In P. A. Mellars & C. B. Stringer (Eds.), The human revolution: Behavioural and biological perspectives on the origin of modern humans (pp. 338365). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Mellars, P. A. (1996). Symbolism, language, and the Neanderthal mind. In P. Mellars & K. R. Gibson (Eds.), Modelling the early human mind (pp. 1532). Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

Mellars, P. A., & French, J. C. (2011). Tenfold population increase in Western Europe at the Neandertal-to-modern human transition.

Science, 333, 623627.

Mishra, S., Gaillard, C., Hertler, C., Moigne, A.-M., & Simanjuntak, T. (2010). India and Java: Contrasting records, intimate connections.

Quaternary International, 223224, 265270.

Moore, M. W. (2003a). Australian Aboriginal biface reduction technique on the Georgina Rive, Camooweal, Queensland. Australian Archaeology, 56, 2234.

Moore, M. W. (2003b). Australian Aboriginal blade production methods on the Georgina River, Camooweal, Queensland. Lithic Technology, 28, 2754.

Moore, M. W. (2003c). Flexibility of stone tool manufacturing methods on the Georgina River, Camooweal, Queensland. Archaeology in Oceania, 38, 2336.

Moore, M. W. (2010). Grammars of actionand stone aking design space. In A. Nowell & I. Davidson (Eds.), Stone tools and the evolution of human cognition (pp. 1343). Boulder, CO: University Press of Colorado.

Moore, M. W. (2011). The design space of stone aking: Implications for cognitive evolution. World Archaeology, 43, 702715.

Moore, M. W. (2013). Simple stone aking in Australasia: Patterns and implications. Quaternary International, 285, 140149.

Moore, M. W., & Brumm, A. (2007). Stone artifacts and hominins in Island Southeast Asia: New insights from Flores, Eastern Indonesia. [Review]. Journal of Human Evolution, 52, 85102.

Nishida, T. (1986). Local traditions and cultural transmission. In B. B. Smuts, D. L. Cheney, R. M. Seyfarth, R. W. Wrangham, & T. T. Struhsaker (Eds.), Primate societies (pp. 462474). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

Noble, W., & Davidson, I. (1996). Human evolution, language and mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Nowell, A. (2010). Working memory and the speed of life. Current Anthropology, 51, S121S133.

Nowell, A. (2016). Childhood, play and the evolution of cultural capacity in Neanderthals and Modern Humans. In M. N. Haidle, N. J. Conard, & M. Bolus (Eds.), The nature of culture (pp. 8797). Dordrecht: Springer.

Nowell, A., & Davidson, I. (Eds.). (2010). Stone tools and the evolution of human cognition. Boulder, CO: University Press of Colorado.

Nowell, A., & White, M. J. (2010). Middle Pleistocene life history strategies and their relationship to Acheulean industries. In A. Nowell & I. Davidson (Eds.), Stone tools and the evolution of human cognition. Boulder, CO: University Press of Colorado.

Perry, S. (2011). Social traditions and social learning in capuchin monkeys (Cebus). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 988996.

Pope, M. I. (2002). The signicance of biface-rich assemblages: An examination of the behavioural controls on lithic assemblage formation in the Lower Palaeolithic. University of Southampton, Unpublished Ph.D. Thesis.

Pope, M. I., Russel, K., & Watson, K. (2006). Biface form and structured behaviour in the Acheulean. Lithics, 27, 4457.

Prasad, K. N. (1982). Was Ramapithecus a tool-user? Journal of Human Evolution, 11, 101104.

Rainey, A. (1991). Some Australian bifaces. Lithics, 12, 3336. Rappaport, R. A. (1999). Ritual and religion in the making of humanity.

Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Rendell, L., & Whitehead, H. (2001). Cultures in whales and dolphins.

Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 309382.

Reynolds, S. C., Bailey, G. N., & King, G. C. P. (2011). Landscapes and their relation to hominin habitats: Case studies from Australopithecus sites in Eastern and Southern Africa. Journal of Human Evolution, 60, 281298.

Roberts, M. B., Partt, S. A., Pope, M. I., & Wenban-Smith, F. F. (1997). Boxgrove, West Sussex: Rescue excavations of a lower Palaeolitic landsurface. Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society, 63, 303358.

Roebroeks, W., Kolen, J., & Rensink, E. (1988). Planning depth, anticipation and the organization of Middle Palaeolithic technology: The archaic nativemeet Eves descendants. Helinium, 28, 1734.

Rolland, N., & Dibble, H. L. (1990). A new synthesis of Middle Paleolithic variability. American Antiquity, 55, 480499.

Ross, J., & Davidson, I. (2006). Rock art and ritual: An archaeological analysis of rock art in arid Central Australia. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 13, 305341.

Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Williams, S. L., Furuichi, T., & Kano, T. (1996). Language perceived: Paniscus branches out. In W. C. McGrew, L. F. Marchant, & T. Nishida (Eds.), Great ape societies (pp. 173184). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Fields, W. M., & Spircu, T. (2004). The emergence of knapping and vocal expression embedded in a Pan/Homo culture. Biology and Philosophy, 19, 541575.

Schäfer, J. (1990). Conjoining of artefacts and consideration of raw material. Their application at the Middle Palaeolithic site of Schweinskopf-Karmelenberg. In E. Cziesla, S. Eickhoff, N. Arts, & D. Winter (Eds.), The Big Puzzle. Proceedings of the international symposium on retting stone artefacts (pp. 83100). Bonn: Holos.

Schlanger, N. (1996). Understanding Levallois: Lithic technology and cognitive Archaeology. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 6, 231254.

Sharon, G. (2008). The impact of raw material on Acheulian large ake production. Journal of Archaeological Science, 35, 13291344.

Sharon, G. (2010). Large ake Acheulian. Quaternary International, 223224, 226233.

Shennan, S. J., & Steele, J. (1999). Cultural learning in hominids: A behavioural ecological approach. In H. O. Box & K. R. Gibson (Eds.),

Mammalian social learning. Comparative and ecological perspectives

(pp. 367388). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Stiner, M. C. (1994). Honor among thieves. A zooarchaeological study of Neandertal Ecology. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Stiner, M. C. (2006). Middle Paleolithic subsistence ecology of the Mediterranean region. In E. Hovers & S. Kuhn (Eds.), Transitions before the transition. Evolution and stability in the Middle Paleolithic and the Middle Stone Age (pp. 213231). New York: Springer.

Stiner, M. C., & Kuhn, S. L. (1992). Subsistence, technology, and adaptive variation in Middle Palaeolithic Italy. American Anthropologist, 94, 306339.

Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 24052415.

120

I. Davidson

Tennie, C., Braun, D. R., Premo, L. S., & McPherron, S. P. (2016). The Island test for cumulative culture in the Paleolithic. In M. N. Haidle, N. J. Conard, & M. Bolus (Eds.), The nature of culture (pp. 121131). Dordrecht: Springer.

Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Toth, N. (1985). The Oldowan reassessed: A close look at early stone artifacts. Journal of Archaeological Science, 12, 101120.

Trinkaus, E., & Shipman, P. (1993). The Neandertals. Changing the image of mankind. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.

Tylor, E. B. (1870). Primitive culture. Researches into the development of mythology, philosophy, religion, language, art, and custom. London: John Murray.

Van Peer, P. (1992). The Levallois reduction strategy. Madison, Wisconsin: Prehistory Press.

van Schaik, C. P., & Pradhan, G. R. (2003). A model for tool-use traditions in primates: Implications for the coevolution of culture and cognition. Journal of Human Evolution, 44, 645664.

van Schaik, C. P., Ancrenaz, M., Borgen, G., Galdikas, B., Knott, C. D., Singleton, I., et al. (2003). Orangutan cultures and the evolution of material culture. Science, 299, 102105.

Vandermeersch, B. (1981). Les hommes fossiles de Qafzeh (Israël). Paris: CNRS.

Visalberghi, E., & Fragaszy, D. (1990). Do monkeys ape? In S. Taylor Parker & K. R. Gibson (Eds.), Languageand intelligence in monkeys and apes (pp. 247273). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

White, L. A. (1959). The evolution of culture. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Whiten, A., & Boesch, C. (2001). The cultures of chimpanzees.

Scientic American, 284(1), 4855.

Whiten, A., & van Schaik, C. (2007). The evolution of animal culturesand social intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362, 603620.

Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., et al. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682685.

Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., et al. (2001). Charting cultural variation in chimpanzees. Behaviour, 138, 14811516.

Whiten, A., Horner, V., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2003). Cultural panthropology. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 92105.

Wiessner, P. (1983). Style and social information in Kalahari San projectile points. American Antiquity, 48, 253276.

Wynn, T., & Coolidge, F. (2010). How Levallois reduction is similar to, and not similar to, playing chess. In A. Nowell & I. Davidson (Eds.), Stone tools and the evolution of human cognition. Boulder, CO: University Press of Colorado.

Wynn, T., & McGrew, W. C. (1989). An apes eye view of the Oldowan. Man, 24, 383398.

Wynn, T., Hernandez-Aguilar, R. A., Marchant, L. F., & McGrew, W. C. (2011). An apes view of the Oldowanrevisited. Evolutionary Anthropology, 20, 181197.

Zhang, P., Huang, W., & Wang, W. (2010). Acheulean handaxes from Fengshudao, Bose sites of South China. Quaternary International, 223224, 440443.