Добавил:
Опубликованный материал нарушает ваши авторские права? Сообщите нам.
Вуз: Предмет: Файл:
The Nature of Culture.pdf
Скачиваний:
29
Добавлен:
11.11.2021
Размер:
6.21 Mб
Скачать

8 Group Identity in Early Upper Paleolithic Tools

85

discussion about styleversus typesin mind, I remain pessimistic about the possibility to unambiguously trace group identity in the early Upper Paleolithic.

Acknowledgements I thank Miriam Haidle, Jürgen Richter, and Thorsten Uthmeier for very helpful comments.

References

Barth, M. (2007). Familienbande? Die gravettienzeitlichen Knochenund Geweihgeräte des Achtals (Schwäbische Alb). Tübinger Arbeiten zur Urgeschichte 4. Rahden/Westf.: Verlag Marie Leidorf.

Barton, C. M. (1997). Stone tools, style, and social identity: An evolutionary perspective on the archaeological record. Archaeological Papers of the American Anthropological Association, 7, 141156.

Bar-Yosef, O. (2002). The Upper Paleolithic revolution. Annual Review of Anthropology, 31, 363391.

Benazzi, S., Douka, K., Fornai, C., Bauer, C. C., Kullmer, O., Svoboda, J., et al. (2011). Early dispersal of modern humans in Europe and implications for Neanderthal behaviour. Nature, 479, 525529.

Bolus, M. (2004). Der Übergang vom Mittelzum Jungpaläolithikum in Europa. Eine Bestandsaufnahme unter besonderer Berücksichtigung Mitteleuropas. Germania, 82, 154.

Bolus, M. (2010). Continuity or Hiatus? The Swabian Aurignacian and the transition to the Gravettian. In C. Neugebauer Maresch & L. R. Owen (Eds.), New Aspects of the Central and Eastern European Upper Palaeolithic methods, chronology, technology and subsistence (pp. 139150). Wien: Verlag der Österreichischen Akademie der Wissenschaften.

Bolus, M., & Conard, N. J. (2006). Zur Zeitstellung von Geschossspitzen aus organischen Materialien im späten Mittelpaläolithikum und Aurignacien. Archäologisches Korrespondenzblatt, 36, 115.

Bolus, M., & Conard, N. J. (2008). What can we say about the spatial-temporal distribution of early Aurignacian innovations?

Eurasian Prehistory, 5(2), 1929.

Broglio, A., Bertola, S., de Stefani, M., & Marini, D. (2002). LAurignaziano della Grotta di Fumane. In A. Aspes (Ed.),

Preistoria Veronese. Contributi e Aggiornamenti (pp. 2936). Memorie del Museo Civico di Storia Naturale di Verona 2A Serie. Sezione Scienze dellUomo 5. Verona: Museo Civico di Storia Naturale di Verona Museo Tridentino di Scienze Naturali.

Burmeister, S., & Müller-Scheeßel, N. (Eds.) (2006). Soziale Gruppen, Kulturelle Grenzen. Die Interpretation sozialer Identitäten in der Prähistorischen Archäologie. Tübinger Archäologische Taschen- bücher 5. Münster, New York, München & Berlin: Waxmann.

Chiotti, L. (2000). Lamelles Dufour et grattoirs aurignaciens (carénés et à museau) de la couche 8 de labri Pataud, Les Eyzies-de-Tayac, Dordogne. LAnthropologie, 104, 239263.

Close, A. E. (1978). The identication of style in lithic artefacts. World Archaeology, 10, 223237.

Conard, N. J. (2003). Eiszeitlicher Schmuck auf der Schwäbischen Alb. In S. Kölbl & N. J. Conard (Eds.), Eiszeitschmuck Status und Schönheit (pp. 1549). Blaubeuren: Urgeschichtliches Museum.

Conard, N. J. (2008). A critical view of the evidence for a Southern African origin of behavioural modernity. South African Archaeological Society Goodwin Series, 10, 175179.

Conard, N. J., & Bolus, M. (2015). Chronicling modern humans arrival in Europe. Science, 348, 754756.

Hahn, J. (1988). Die Geißenklösterle-Höhle im Achtal bei Blaubeuren I. Fundhorizontbildung und Besiedlung im Mittelpaläolithikum und im Aurignacien. With contributions by

H. Gollnisch, A. Scheer, N. Symens, R. Whallon, & J. Weißhaupt. Stuttgart: Theiss.

Haidle, M. N., Bolus, M., Collard, M., Conard, N. J., Garofoli, D., Lombard, M., et al. (2015). The nature of culture An eight-grade model for the evolution and expansion of cultural capacities in hominins and other animals. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 93, 4370.

Koehler, H. (2009). Comportements et identité techniques au Paléolithique moyen (Weichsélien ancien) dans le Bassin parisien: une question d’échelle danalyse? Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Paris-Nanterre. Published electronically. http://www.theses.fr/ 2009PA100185/document/.

Laplace, G. (1966). Recherches sur lorigine et l’évolution des complexes leptolithiques. Paris: Éditions de Boccard.

Lombard, M. (2011). Quartz-tipped arrows older than 60 ka: Further use-trace evidence from Sibudu, KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa.

Journal of Archaeological Science, 38, 19181930.

Lombard, M., & Haidle, M. N. (2012). Thinking a bow-and-arrow set: Cognitive implications of Middle Stone Age bow and stone-tipped arrow technology. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 22(2), 237264.

Lombard, M., & Phillipson, L. (2010). Indications of bow and stone-tipped arrow use 64 000 years ago in KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Antiquity, 84, 635648.

Moreau, L. (2009). Geißenklösterle. Das Gravettien der Schwäbischen Alb im europäischen Kontext. Tübingen: Kerns.

Müller-Scheeßel, N., & Burmeister, S. (2006). Einführung: Die Identizierung sozialer Gruppen. Die Erkenntnismöglichkeiten der Prähistorischen Archäologie auf dem Prüfstand. In S. Burmeister & N. Müller-Scheeßel (Eds.), Soziale Gruppen, Kulturelle Grenzen. Die Interpretation sozialer Identitäten in der Prähistorischen Archäologie (pp. 938). Tübinger Archäologische Taschenbücher 5. Münster, New York, München & Berlin: Waxmann.

Nigst, P. R. (2009). The Early Upper Palaeolithic in the Middle Danube region: A regional study using the evidence of Willendorf II, Stratzing 94, Vedrovice V, and Stránská skála IIIc. Ph.D. thesis, University of Leipzig (published version 2012: The Early Upper Palaeolithic in the Middle Danube Region. Leiden: Leiden University Press).

Palma di Cesnola, A. (1989). LUluzzien: faciès italien du Leptolithique archaïque. LAnthropologie, 93, 783811.

Sackett, J. R. (1982). Approaches to style in lithic archaeology. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 1, 59112.

Uthmeier, T. (2016). Neanderthal utilitarian equipment and group identity: The social context of bifacial tool manufacture and use. In M. N. Haidle, N. J. Conard, & M. Bolus (Eds.), The nature of culture (pp. 6577). Dordrecht: Springer.

Vanhaeren, M., & dErrico, F. (2006). Aurignacian ethno-linguistic geography of Europe revealed by personal ornaments. Journal of Archaeological Science, 33, 11051128.

Villa, P., Soriano, S., Teyssandier, N., & Wurz, S. (2010). The Howiesons Poort and MSA III at Klasies River main site, Cave 1A.

Journal of Archaeological Science, 37, 630655.

Wadley, L. (2003). How some archaeologists recognize culturally modern behavior. South African Journal of Science, 99, 247250.

Wiessner, P. (1983). Style and social information in Kalahari San projectile points. American Antiquity, 48, 253276.

Wobst, H. M. (1977). Stylistic behavior and information exchange. In C. E. Cleland (Ed.), For the director: Research essays in honor of James B. Grifn (pp. 317342). Anthropological paper 61. Ann Arbor: Museum of Anthropology, University of Michigan.

Wolf, S. (2015). Schmuckstücke. Die Elfenbeinbearbeitung im Schwäbischen Aurignacien. Tübingen: Kerns Verlag.

Wurz, S. (2008). Modern behavior at Klasies River. South African Archaeological Society Goodwin Series, 10, 150156.

Chapter 9

Childhood, Play and the Evolution of Cultural Capacity

in Neanderthals and Modern Humans

April Nowell

Abstract The life history pattern of modern humans is characterized by the insertion of childhood and adolescent stages into the typical primate pattern. It is widely recognized that this slowing of the maturational process provides humans with additional years to learn, transmit, practice and modify cultural behaviors. In both human and non-human primates a signicant amount of their respective dependency periods are spent in play. In contrast to modern humans, Neanderthals experienced shorter childhoods. This is significant as there is extensive psychological and neurobiological evidence that it is during infancy, childhood and adolescence that milestones in social and cognitive learning are reached and that play and play deprivation have a direct impact on this development. Faster maturation rates and thus shorter childhoods relative to modern humans lessen the impact of learning through play on the connectivity of the brain. In the context of play behavior, humans are unique in that adult humans play more than adults of any other species and they alone engage in fantasy play. Fantasy play is part of a package of symbol-based cognitive abilities that includes self-awareness, language, and theory of mind. Its benets include creativity, behavioral plasticity, imagination, apprenticeship and planning. Differences in the nature of symbolic material culture of Neanderthals and modern humans suggest that Neanderthals were not capable of engaging in human-grade fantasy play.

Keywords Middle Paleolithic Upper Paleolithic Life history Brain Behavioral plasticity Cognitive development Fantasy Imagination Archaeology of children

A. Nowell (&)

Department of Anthropology, University of Victoria, STN CSC, P.O. Box 3050, Victoria, BC V8W 3P5, Canada

e-mail: anowell@uvic.ca

Introduction

In prehistoric societies children likely comprised at least forty to sixty-ve percent of the population (Baxter 2005, 2008), yet the archaeological literature, if not the archaeological record itself, has largely been silent about the lives they lived and the contributions they made (e.g., see discussions in Sofaer Derevenski 1997; Kamp 2001; Shea 2006). Childrens play and their unconventional use of material culture were, until recently, believed to introduce a randomizing and distorting element into the archaeological record (Baxter 2005, 2008). Children were not only unknown, they were unknowable. This chapter, however, argues that childrens play is in fact serious business and that an understanding of how experiences garnered in childhood shape the brain may be key to documenting the evolution of unique aspects of human cognition and behavior. While this chapter is not about ‘findingchildren and their material culture in the Paleolithic record per se it does focus on childhood as a crucial ontological stage for cognitive maturation and on the implications for behavior and cultural capacity in having a longer period of neural development.

This chapter begins by describing the slowing of the human maturation process and the emergence of childhood and adolescence stages in human life history. Then, maturation rates and lengths of childhood are compared between Neanderthals and modern humans based on game changing studies of dental, brain and somatic growth and development. Next, extensive psychological and neurobiological evidence is presented that suggests that it is during infancy, childhood and adolescence that milestones in social and cognitive learning are reached and that play and play deprivation have a direct impact on this development. By way of discussion and conclusion, it is argued that Neanderthals had shorter childhoods and this inuenced the adults they became and the material culture they produced. It is further argued that the key to understanding hominin

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2016

87

Miriam N. Haidle, Nicholas J. Conard and Michael Bolus (eds.), The Nature of Culture:

Based on an Interdisciplinary Symposium The Nature of Culture, Tübingen, Germany,

Vertebrate Paleobiology and Paleoanthropology, DOI 10.1007/978-94-017-7426-0_9

88

A. Nowell

cognitive abilities lies not in absolute brain size or encephalization quotients but rather in our ability to reconstruct the childhoods of our hominin ancestors and to use the biological and archaeological evidence to piece together how they spent that time.

Human Life History

Life history can be dened as the allocation of an organisms energy for growth, maintenance and reproduction[and is] a life strategy adopted by an organism to maximize tness in a world of limited energy(Dean and Smith 2009:115). Life history is sometimes referred to as reproductive turnover or the speed of life(Stearns 1992; Nowell 2010). Following this metaphor, it is often said that primates have the slowest life histories of all the mammals (Harvey and Clutton-Brock 1985; Zimmermann and Radespiel 2007; Robson and Wood 2008) and, by extension, one might infer that humans experience the slowest life histories of all the primates but the emerging picture of human life history is at once more complex and more elegant (Nowell 2010).

As paleoanthropologists, we are accustomed to thinking in terms of k-strategists and r-strategists. Relative to r-strategists, k-strategists reach the age of reproduction more slowly, have fewer numbers of offspring, and put greater investment in each offspring. While k-strategists are thought to produce high quality offspring, the drawback to being a slow reproducer is that you risk dying before you reproduce (Robson and Wood 2008; Dean and Smith 2009:101). But humans are unique in that they have elements of an r-strategy within the larger life history of a k-strategist. Specically, they have a long gestation period, a large brain, mature more slowly with females reaching the age of reproduction later, experience an extended dependency period, and enjoy increased longevity, but at the same time they have evolved shorter birth spacing, they wean sooner, and have more dependents than expected for an ape that matures at the age modern humans do (Robson and Wood 2008). Essentially, modern humans are characterized by a reproductive pattern that works twice as fast as that of the great apes (Dean and Smith 2009:115) something that Wood (1994) has described as secondary r-selection. Added to this unique combination of features are a suite of derived elements concealed ovulation, helpless young, rapid postnatal brain growth in infants, continued dependency after weaning, paternal care, and vigorous post-menopausal life in females (Bogin 1997; Kaplan 2002; Hawkes et al. 2003; Leigh 2004, 2012; Zimmermann and Radespiel 2007; Dean and Smith 2009:115; Robson and Wood 2008). Thus, modern humans have developed a strategy of producing high quality offspring while at the same time reducing the risk of

dying before maturation by living fastin some respects, within an overall pattern of living slow.

This slowing of the human maturation process relative to other primates has led to the development of two unique life history stages in humans. Human biologists divide human life history into ve stages infancy (from birth to weaning), childhood (from weaning to the eruption of M1), juvenile (from M1 to puberty and the onset of the adolescent growth spurt), adolescence (from the onset of the adolescent growth spurt to the cessation of the growth and maturation) and adulthood (Mace 2000; Bogin 2003, 2009) with the stages of childhood and adolescence being unique to humans. It should be noted that the term childhood (and adolescence for that matter) is often thought of as a cultural construct and depending on the context it can be. We know that crossculturally and over time what it means phenomenologically to be a childvaries greatly (e.g., Kamp 2001; Baxter 2008; Konner 2010) but the terms childhoodand adolescenceare used here as biologically dened stages of human development dependent on anatomical markers that permit comparisons between species.

The slowing of the maturation process in humans, as Bogin (2003:32) notes, is signicant as the childhood stage adds an additional four years of relatively slow growth and allows for behavioral experience that further enhances developmental plasticity(see also Neubauer and Hublin 2012) while adolescence further extends this period of growth and development with additional years to learn, practice, transmit, modify and innovate upon aspects of their culture. Data from pediatric fMRI studies of prefrontal cortical activity suggest that attention, memory and inhibition all key elements of working memory continue to develop during childhood and adolescence (Casey et al. 2000) with the dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex, the region most closely associated with working memory, the last region of the brain to mature and develop (Casey et al. 2000; Vuontela et al. 2003; Paus 2005; Durston and Casey 2006; Neubauer and Hublin 2012).

Childhood in Neanderthals and Modern

Humans

Current data suggest that while Neanderthals matured more slowly than earlier hominins such as Homo erectus, they matured more quickly than modern humans and experienced a shorter childhood as a result. For instance, Smith et al. (2010) concluded based on a synchrotron virtual histology study of a Middle Paleolithic dental sample from Neanderthal and early modern human juveniles that Neanderthals matured more quickly than their modern human counterparts do. They found that both have an extended duration of