
- •1.Scientific Paradigms. Thomas Kuhn's Paradigm shift
- •2.Historical Linguistics. Structural Linguistics
- •3.Structural Linguistics. European and American Structuralism
- •4.Structural Linguistics. Anthropological linguistics
- •5. Anthropological linguistics
- •6.'Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis'
- •7.'Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis' vs Slobin's 'thinking for speaking' theory
- •8.Cultural linguistics. Language and culture
- •9.Universal human concepts
- •10.The ration thinkers' idea on universal human concepts (Leibniz, Descartes, Pascal)
- •12.Key words and core cultural values
- •13.Human universal concepts. The Concept Friendship
- •14.Cultural Universalism
- •15.Cultural relativism
- •16.Cultural Universal. Cultural relativism
- •17.The Natural Semantic Metalanguage Theory
- •18.Anna Wierzbicka's 'Semantic primitives'
- •19.The Theory of Cultural Scripts
7.'Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis' vs Slobin's 'thinking for speaking' theory
The Sapir-Whorf theory, named after the American linguists Edward Sapir and Benjamin Lee Whorf, is a mould theory of language. Writing in 1929, Sapir argued in a classic passage that:
1)Human beings do not live in the objective world alone, nor alone in the world of social activity as ordinarily understood, but are very much at the mercy of the particular language which has become the medium of expression for their society. It is quite an illusion to imagine that one adjusts to reality essentially without the use of language and that language is merely an incidental means of solving specific problems of communication or reflection. The fact of the matter is that the 'real world' is to a large extent unconsciously built upon the language habits of the group. No two languages are ever sufficiently similar to be considered as representing the same social reality. The worlds in which different societies live are distinct worlds, not merely the same world with different labels attached... We see and hear and otherwise experience very largely as we do because the language habits of our community predispose certain choices of interpretation.
This position was extended in the 1930s by his student Whorf, who, in another widely cited passage, declared that:
2)We dissect nature along lines laid down by our native languages. The categories and types that we isolate from the world of phenomena we do not find there because they stare every observer in the face; on the contrary, the world is presented in a kaleidoscopic flux of impressions which has to be organized by our minds - and this means largely by the linguistic systems in our minds. We cut nature up, organize it into concepts, and ascribe significances as we do, largely because we are parties to an agreement to organize it in this way - an agreement that holds throughout our speech community and is codified in the patterns of our language. The agreement is, of course, an implicit and unstated one, but its terms are absolutely obligatory; we cannot talk at all except by subscribing to the organization and classification of data which the agreement decrees
Daniel Isaac Slobin (born 1939) is a Professor Emeritus of psychology and linguistics at the University of California, Berkeley. Slobin has extensively studied the organization of information about spatial relations and motion events by speakers of different languages, including both children and adults. He has argued that becoming a competent speaker of a language requires learning certain language-specific modes of thinking, which he dubbed "thinking for speaking". Slobin's "thinking for speaking" view can be described as a contemporary, moderate version of the Sapir–Whorf hypothesis, which claims that the language we learn shapes the way we perceive reality and think about it. This view is often contrasted with the "language acquisition device" view of Noam Chomsky and others, who think of language acquisition as a process largely independent of learning and cognitive development.