
- •Contents
- •1 Russian
- •1.1 The Russian language
- •1.1.1 Russian then and now
- •1.1.2 Levels of language
- •1.2 Describing Russian grammar
- •1.2.1 Conventions of notation
- •1.2.2 Abbreviations
- •1.2.3 Dictionaries and grammars
- •1.2.4 Statistics and corpora
- •1.2.5 Strategies of describing Russian grammar
- •1.2.6 Two fundamental concepts of (Russian) grammar
- •1.3 Writing Russian
- •1.3.1 The Russian Cyrillic alphabet
- •1.3.2 A brief history of the Cyrillic alphabet
- •1.3.3 Etymology of letters
- •1.3.4 How the Cyrillic alphabet works (basics)
- •1.3.5 How the Cyrillic alphabet works (refinements)
- •1.3.6 How the Cyrillic alphabet works (lexical idiosyncrasies)
- •1.3.7 Transliteration
- •2 Sounds
- •2.1 Sounds
- •2.2 Vowels
- •2.2.1 Stressed vowels
- •2.2.3 Vowel duration
- •2.2.4 Unstressed vowels
- •2.2.5 Unpaired consonants [ˇs ˇz c] and unstressed vocalism
- •2.2.6 Post-tonic soft vocalism
- •2.2.7 Unstressed vowels in sequence
- •2.2.8 Unstressed vowels in borrowings
- •2.3 Consonants
- •2.3.1 Classification of consonants
- •2.3.2 Palatalization of consonants
- •2.3.3 The distribution of palatalized consonants
- •2.3.4 Palatalization assimilation
- •2.3.5 The glide [j]
- •2.3.6 Affricates
- •2.3.7 Soft palatal fricatives
- •2.3.8 Geminate consonants
- •2.3.9 Voicing of consonants
- •2.4 Phonological variation
- •2.4.1 General
- •2.4.2 Phonological variation: idiomaticity
- •2.4.3 Phonological variation: systemic factors
- •2.4.4 Phonological variation: phonostylistics and Old Muscovite pronunciation
- •2.5 Morpholexical alternations
- •2.5.1 Preliminaries
- •2.5.2 Consonant grades
- •2.5.3 Types of softness
- •2.5.4 Vowel grades
- •2.5.5 Morphophonemic {o}
- •3 Inflectional morphology
- •3.1 Introduction
- •3.2 Conjugation of verbs
- •3.2.1 Verbal categories
- •3.2.2 Conjugation classes
- •3.2.3 Stress patterns
- •3.2.4 Conjugation classes: I-Conjugation
- •3.2.5 Conjugation classes: suffixed E-Conjugation
- •3.2.6 Conjugation classes: quasisuffixed E-Conjugation
- •3.2.7 Stress in verbs: retrospective
- •3.2.8 Irregularities in conjugation
- •3.2.9 Secondary imperfectivization
- •3.3 Declension of pronouns
- •3.3.1 Personal pronouns
- •3.3.2 Third-person pronouns
- •3.3.3 Determiners (demonstrative, possessive, adjectival pronouns)
- •3.4 Quantifiers
- •3.5 Adjectives
- •3.5.1 Adjectives
- •3.5.2 Predicative (‘‘short”) adjectives
- •3.5.3 Mixed adjectives and surnames
- •3.5.4 Comparatives and superlatives
- •3.6 Declension of nouns
- •3.6.1 Categories and declension classes of nouns
- •3.6.2 Hard, soft, and unpaired declensions
- •3.6.3 Accentual patterns
- •3.6.8 Declension and gender of gradation
- •3.6.9 Accentual paradigms
- •3.7 Complications in declension
- •3.7.1 Indeclinable common nouns
- •3.7.2 Acronyms
- •3.7.3 Compounds
- •3.7.4 Appositives
- •3.7.5 Names
- •4 Arguments
- •4.1 Argument phrases
- •4.1.1 Basics
- •4.1.2 Reference of arguments
- •4.1.3 Morphological categories of nouns: gender
- •4.1.4 Gender: unpaired ‘‘masculine” nouns
- •4.1.5 Gender: common gender
- •4.1.6 Morphological categories of nouns: animacy
- •4.1.7 Morphological categories of nouns: number
- •4.1.8 Number: pluralia tantum, singularia tantum
- •4.1.9 Number: figurative uses of number
- •4.1.10 Morphological categories of nouns: case
- •4.2 Prepositions
- •4.2.1 Preliminaries
- •4.2.2 Ligature {o}
- •4.2.3 Case government
- •4.3 Quantifiers
- •4.3.1 Preliminaries
- •4.3.2 General numerals
- •4.3.3 Paucal numerals
- •4.3.5 Preposed quantified noun
- •4.3.6 Complex numerals
- •4.3.7 Fractions
- •4.3.8 Collectives
- •4.3.9 Approximates
- •4.3.10 Numerative (counting) forms of selected nouns
- •4.3.12 Quantifier (numeral) cline
- •4.4 Internal arguments and modifiers
- •4.4.1 General
- •4.4.2 Possessors
- •4.4.3 Possessive adjectives of unique nouns
- •4.4.4 Agreement of adjectives and participles
- •4.4.5 Relative clauses
- •4.4.6 Participles
- •4.4.7 Comparatives
- •4.4.8 Event nouns: introduction
- •4.4.9 Semantics of event nouns
- •4.4.10 Arguments of event nouns
- •4.5 Reference in text: nouns, pronouns, and ellipsis
- •4.5.1 Basics
- •4.5.2 Common nouns in text
- •4.5.3 Third-person pronouns
- •4.5.4 Ellipsis (‘‘zero” pronouns)
- •4.5.5 Second-person pronouns and address
- •4.5.6 Names
- •4.6 Demonstrative pronouns
- •4.7 Reflexive pronouns
- •4.7.1 Basics
- •4.7.2 Autonomous arguments
- •4.7.3 Non-immediate sites
- •4.7.4 Special predicate--argument relations: existential, quantifying, modal, experiential predicates
- •4.7.5 Unattached reflexives
- •4.7.6 Special predicate--argument relations: direct objects
- •4.7.7 Special predicate--argument relations: passives
- •4.7.8 Autonomous domains: event argument phrases
- •4.7.9 Autonomous domains: non-finite verbs
- •4.7.12 Retrospective on reflexives
- •4.8 Quantifying pronouns and adjectives
- •4.8.1 Preliminaries: interrogatives as indefinite pronouns
- •4.8.7 Summary
- •4.8.9 Universal adjectives
- •5 Predicates and arguments
- •5.1 Predicates and arguments
- •5.1.1 Predicates and arguments, in general
- •5.1.2 Predicate aspectuality and modality
- •5.1.3 Aspectuality and modality in context
- •5.1.4 Predicate information structure
- •5.1.5 Information structure in context
- •5.1.6 The concept of subject and the concept of object
- •5.1.7 Typology of predicates
- •5.2 Predicative adjectives and nouns
- •5.2.1 General
- •5.2.2 Modal co-predicates
- •5.2.3 Aspectual co-predicates
- •5.2.4 Aspectual and modal copular predicatives
- •5.2.5 Copular constructions: instrumental
- •5.2.6 Copular adjectives: predicative (short) form vs. nominative (long) form
- •5.2.9 Predicatives in non-finite clauses
- •5.2.10 Summary: case usage in predicatives
- •5.3 Quantifying predicates and genitive subjects
- •5.3.1 Basics
- •5.3.2 Clausal quantifiers and subject quantifying genitive
- •5.3.3 Subject quantifying genitive without quantifiers
- •5.3.4 Existential predication and the subject genitive of negation: basic paradigm
- •5.3.5 Existential predication and the subject genitive of negation: predicates
- •5.3.6 Existential predication and the subject genitive of negation: reference
- •5.3.8 Existential predication and the subject genitive of negation: predicates and reference
- •5.3.9 Existential predication and the subject genitive of negation: context
- •5.3.10 Existential predication and the subject genitive of negation: summary
- •5.4 Quantified (genitive) objects
- •5.4.1 Basics
- •5.4.2 Governed genitive
- •5.4.3 Partitive and metric genitive
- •5.4.4 Object genitive of negation
- •5.4.5 Genitive objects: summary
- •5.5 Secondary genitives and secondary locatives
- •5.5.1 Basics
- •5.5.2 Secondary genitive
- •5.5.3 Secondary locative
- •5.6 Instrumental case
- •5.6.1 Basics
- •5.6.2 Modal instrumentals
- •5.6.3 Aspectual instrumentals
- •5.6.4 Agentive instrumentals
- •5.6.5 Summary
- •5.7 Case: context and variants
- •5.7.1 Jakobson’s case system: general
- •5.7.2 Jakobson’s case system: the analysis
- •5.7.3 Syncretism
- •5.7.4 Secondary genitive and secondary locative as cases?
- •5.8 Voice: reflexive verbs, passive participles
- •5.8.1 Basics
- •5.8.2 Functional equivalents of passive
- •5.8.3 Reflexive verbs
- •5.8.4 Present passive participles
- •5.8.5 Past passive participles
- •5.8.6 Passives and near-passives
- •5.9 Agreement
- •5.9.1 Basics
- •5.9.2 Agreement with implicit arguments, complications
- •5.9.3 Agreement with overt arguments: special contexts
- •5.9.4 Agreement with conjoined nouns
- •5.9.5 Agreement with comitative phrases
- •5.9.6 Agreement with quantifier phrases
- •5.10 Subordinate clauses and infinitives
- •5.10.1 Basics
- •5.10.2 Finite clauses
- •5.10.4 The free infinitive construction (without overt modal)
- •5.10.5 The free infinitive construction (with negative existential pronouns)
- •5.10.6 The dative-with-infinitive construction (overt modal)
- •5.10.7 Infinitives with modal hosts (nominative subject)
- •5.10.8 Infinitives with hosts of intentional modality (nominative subject)
- •5.10.9 Infinitives with aspectual hosts (nominative subject)
- •5.10.10 Infinitives with hosts of imposed modality (accusative or dative object)
- •5.10.11 Final constructions
- •5.10.12 Summary of infinitive constructions
- •6 Mood, tense, and aspect
- •6.1 States and change, times, alternatives
- •6.2 Mood
- •6.2.1 Modality in general
- •6.2.2 Mands and the imperative
- •6.2.3 Conditional constructions
- •6.2.4 Dependent irrealis mood: possibility, volitive, optative
- •6.2.5 Dependent irrealis mood: epistemology
- •6.2.6 Dependent irrealis mood: reference
- •6.2.7 Independent irrealis moods
- •6.2.8 Syntax and semantics of modal predicates
- •6.3 Tense
- •6.3.1 Predicates and times, in general
- •6.3.2 Tense in finite adjectival and adverbial clauses
- •6.3.3 Tense in argument clauses
- •6.3.4 Shifts of perspective in tense: historical present
- •6.3.5 Shifts of perspective in tense: resultative
- •6.3.6 Tense in participles
- •6.3.7 Aspectual-temporal-modal particles
- •6.4 Aspect and lexicon
- •6.4.1 Aspect made simple
- •6.4.2 Tests for aspect membership
- •6.4.3 Aspect and morphology: the core strategy
- •6.4.4 Aspect and morphology: other strategies and groups
- •6.4.5 Aspect pairs
- •6.4.6 Intrinsic lexical aspect
- •6.4.7 Verbs of motion
- •6.5 Aspect and context
- •6.5.1 Preliminaries
- •6.5.2 Past ‘‘aoristic” narrative: perfective
- •6.5.3 Retrospective (‘‘perfect”) contexts: perfective and imperfective
- •6.5.4 The essentialist context: imperfective
- •6.5.5 Progressive context: imperfective
- •6.5.6 Durative context: imperfective
- •6.5.7 Iterative context: imperfective
- •6.5.8 The future context: perfective and imperfective
- •6.5.9 Exemplary potential context: perfective
- •6.5.10 Infinitive contexts: perfective and imperfective
- •6.5.11 Retrospective on aspect
- •6.6 Temporal adverbs
- •6.6.1 Temporal adverbs
- •6.6.2 Measured intervals
- •6.6.3 Time units
- •6.6.4 Time units: variations on the basic patterns
- •6.6.14 Frequency
- •6.6.15 Some lexical adverbs
- •6.6.16 Conjunctions
- •6.6.17 Summary
- •7 The presentation of information
- •7.1 Basics
- •7.2 Intonation
- •7.2.1 Basics
- •7.2.2 Intonation contours
- •7.3 Word order
- •7.3.1 General
- •7.3.6 Word order without subjects
- •7.3.7 Summary of word-order patterns of predicates and arguments
- •7.3.8 Emphatic stress and word order
- •7.3.9 Word order within argument phrases
- •7.3.10 Word order in speech
- •7.4 Negation
- •7.4.1 Preliminaries
- •7.4.2 Distribution and scope of negation
- •7.4.3 Negation and other phenomena
- •7.5 Questions
- •7.5.1 Preliminaries
- •7.5.2 Content questions
- •7.5.3 Polarity questions and answers
- •7.6 Lexical information operators
- •7.6.1 Conjunctions
- •7.6.2 Contrastive conjunctions
- •Bibliography
- •Index
388A Reference Grammar of Russian
Grammatical tense in the subordinate clause is determined with respect to the time of speech.
6.3.3 Tense in argument clauses
The third group of subordinate clauses are those that fulfill the functions of nominal arguments. They can be subjects: {zcyj vyt rfpfkjcm vtyz djpvenbkj (nj) cnfkj bpdtcnyj} xnj ‘{it is clear it seemed to me it upset me it became known} that’. Or they can be objects: {pyf/ cxbnf/ ujdjh/ dth/ ljkj;bk}, xnj ‘I {know consider say believe reported} that’. In these instances the main verb reports speech in an extended sense: speech, thought, attitudes, beliefs, knowledge, perceptions, representations. Clauses of embedded speech (in this generous sense) can be said to have two layers of speech and speakers: the speech of the primary, or e x t e r n a l , speaker, as opposed to the speech of the secondary, or i n t e r n a l , speaker. In such situations of nested speech, the internal speaker is closer to the event reported; the external speaker has access to that information only by virtue of being the addressee of the internal speaker. This fact influences how tense is used in such clauses.9
The most general conjunction is xnj ‘that’. With xnj, there are five possibilities of tense-aspect forms in the subordinate clause. Assume that the main verb, which names the act of reporting internal speech, is in the past tense. (a) Then a P A S T P E R F E C T I V E refers to an event completed earlier than the time of internal speech ([81] gj[jhjy∫k):
[81]Tlbr hfccrfpfk<pf pst> , xnj gj[jhjybk<pf pst> ;tye, b pfgkfrfk. Edik told that he had buried his wife, and began to cry.
(b) A P A S T I M P E R F E C T I V E refers to a state or activity that occurred prior to the time of the internal speech (in [82], a prior conversation is discussed on the way home):
[82]Gj ljhjut jy hfccrfpfk vyt, xnj d rf,bytnt e Cfdbyrjdf htxm ikf<if pst> j rfrjq-nj nhtnmtq Hjccbb.
On the way he told me that the discussion at Savinkov’s had been about the so-called Third Russia.
(c) An event expressed as a P E R F E C T I V E P R E S E N T is future relative to the time of internal speech ([83]), (d) as is a F U T U R E I M P E R F E C T I V E ([84]):
9The variation has been recognized and documented by Boeck (1957, 1958, source of [94]) and more recently Barentsen (1996) (especially for clauses with rfr). For other (not identical) views, see Brecht 1975, Comrie 1986[b]. Declerck’s analysis of English tense (1991) can be adapted to Russian.
Mood, tense, and aspect 389
[83]Fcz ,skf edthtyf, xnj vjz vfvf yt cjukfcbncz<pf prs> . Asia was convinced my mother would not agree.
[84]Njulf z lfkf ct,t ckjdj, xnj ,jkmit cfkatnjr dsibdfnm yt ,ele<fut> .
At that time I promised myself that I would never embroider napkins again.
(e) The fifth tense-aspect form is the P R E S E N T - T E N S E I M P E R F E C T I V E , which refers to an event that holds on the occasion (time and world) of the internal speech:
[85]Dcrjht dct hf,jxbt gjyzkb, xnj hf,jnf/n<if prs> jyb, d ceoyjcnb, ,tcgkfnyj, njkmrj pf gftr.
Soon all the workers understood that they were working, in fact, for no pay, just for rations.
[86]Reghby lhtvfk gthtl gecnjq ,enskrjq, f vj;tn ,snm, ghbndjhzkcz, xnj lhtvktn<if prs> .
Kuprin dozed in front of an empty bottle, or possibly, pretended that he was dozing.
In [85], the work includes the time of understanding, and in [86], the dozing overlaps the secondary speech event (Kuprin’s dissimulation). It might be noted that, according to the “sequence of tense rule,” the embedded verb in English would have an additional mark of past tense, reflecting the fact that the internal speech is embedded under a past verb.
Indirect questions determine tense relative to the time of the matrix clause in a similar fashion. In [87] the present-tense question concerns a situation at the same time as the question was posed. In [88], the questions are localized relative to the time of imagination.
[87]K/,jdm Vb[fqkjdyf cghjcbkf, rfr tve ;bdtncz<if prs> d Njvcrt. Liubov Mikhailovna asked how he was getting along in Tomsk.
[88]Yt vjukb ghtlcnfdbnm ct,t -- rjuj jyb edblzn<pf prs> , rfrjq jy cnfk<pf pst> ? They could not imagine -- who would they see, how had he changed?
The same principle -- tense in the subordinate clause is determined with respect to the time of the internal speech event -- holds when the matrix verb is a subjunctive or future. In [89], the pin-swallowing is predicted to occur after the shouting begins, and this projected act is expressed by a perfective present referring to the future.
[89]Tckb , z dpzkf ,ekfdre d hjn, cj dct[ cnjhjy cnfkb ,s<irr> rhbxfnm, xnj ctqxfc z ghjukjxe<pf prs> tt b vyt ghbltncz<pf prs> ltkfnm jgthfwb/.
If I should put a pin in my mouth, they would cry out from all sides that I am just about to swallow it and I’ll have to have an operation.
390 A Reference Grammar of Russian
[90]Rjulf ,kbpytws epyf/n<pf prst> , xnj gjzdbkcz<pf pst> rfrjq-nj <j,br, jyb ,elen hsxfnm.
When the twins find out that some Bobik has appeared, they’ll growl.
In [90], Bobik’s appearance is past tense because it occurs before the future awareness of it.
Thus, as a rule, clauses conveying speech (intelligence, speech, knowledge, etc.) -- those introduced by xnj or indirect questions with no conjunction -- determine tense in the embedded clause in relation to the time at which the internal speech occurs rather than in relation to the here and now of the primary speech.
In addition to xnj, Russian also uses the interrogative rfr ‘how’ as a conjunction specifically with verbs of perception: yf,k/lfnm, rfr ‘observe how’; dbltnm, rfr ‘see how’. Verbs of perception are also verbs of speech, in the broad sense: the external speaker has access to information about the world only through the observations and perceptions of the internal speaker. But with rfr, unlike with xnj, the time of the secondary speech is tightly constrained; whatever is observed must hold at the time of observation. Thus a past perfective event is encompassed by the interval of observation:
[91]Z dbltkf, rfr jy d rjhbljht eobgyek<pf pst> d jlyj vtcnj ukege/ Rfn/. I saw how he pinched stupid Katia in a certain place.
The interesting fact is that imperfectives in the subordinate clause introduced by rfr can be either past or present. The present tense reports an activity that is viewed from the perspective of the internal speaker (the observer), as it is in progress; what is of interest is how the activity proceeds, such as how the horse moved ([92]):
[92]B jyf edbltkf, rfr gj ljhjut jn ktcf ljdjkmyj [jlrj tltn<if prs> kjiflm. She saw how a horse was going at a good clip on the road out of the forest.
In contrast, the past imperfective with rfr focuses on the fact that the event occurs at all. It is appropriate if the interval of observation encompasses the event ([93] -- they kiss for a while but stop) or if the event of observation is displaced to the distant past ([94]).
[93]Z dbltkf, rfr jyb wtkjdfkbcm<if pst> . I saw how they kissed.
[94]Jyb pyfkb, xnj gktyysq [jhjij gjybvftn heccrbq zpsr, vyjubt cksifkb, rfr tuj ljghfibdfk<if pst> rjvfylbh gjkrf.
They knew that the prisoner understood Russian well, many of them had heard how the regiment commander had interrogated him.
Mood, tense, and aspect 391
With rfr embedded under verbs of observation, the past imperfective is quite frequent, as much as half of the tokens for some authors.
In fact, the past imperfective can also be used with the conjunction xnj. Although the present tense is usual, a past imperfective is possible if the event is localized to a specific moment in the past ([95]) or the whole situation lies in the remote past ([96]):10
[95]Jy gjyzk, xnj d эnjn vjvtyn Gtnhjd yt ckeifk<if pst> tuj.
He understood that at that moment Petrov wasn’t listening to him.
[96]Vjukf kb z pyfnm, xnj rfr hfp d nt lyb ,sk<pst> ghtlfy cele b j;blfk<if pst> cvthnyjuj ghbujdjhf j,dbytyysq d gjlujnjdrt gjreitybz yf ;bpym Ybrjkfz
ÈÈ by;tyth Pbkm,th,thu, hjlyjq ,hfn vjtq uthjbyb?
Could I possibly have known that exactly in those days an engineer named Zilberberg (the brother of my heroine) was in the hands of the court and was awaiting the death sentence in connection with the attempt on the life of Nicholas II?
In [96], the narrator takes two steps into the past: from the present (when she writes) to her memories of †migr† life in Paris in the twenties, and from there back further in time to the turbulent life of 1906; the memory is buried deep in the past.
The past imperfective is usual when the internal speech (observation) repeats ([97--98]):
[97]Byjulf pf[jlbk Cfif. Jy c djc[botybtv gjcvfnhbdfk yf Yfnfkre, b Cthtuf pfvtxfk, xnj jyf ghb эnjv jgecrfkf<if pst> ukfpf.
Sometimes Sasha would drop in. He would look with admiration at Natalka, and Serega would notice that she would lower her eyes at this.
Iterative contexts presuppose that there is a series of discrete sub-events. Each sub-event involves a definitive change and, as a single event, would be expressed as the past perfective (Cthtuf pfvtnbk, rfr jyf jgecnbkf<pf pst> ukfpf ‘Serega noticed how she lowered her eyes’).11 This past is carried over when the situation is iterated.
Table 6.1 summarizes the conditions for using the present imperfective as opposed to past imperfective for events understood to be simultaneous with past-tense verbs of speech. Generalizing, we can say that the past is possible (with rfr, likely) when the external speaker presents a past situation as limited in validity to a time and world that is anterior to -- and more than that, is distinct from -- the time and world of the external speaker. Using the past imperfective
10Boeck’s observation (1957, 1958), labeled “synchronization” in Timberlake 1982.
11Boeck (1957), Timberlake (1982).