- •Contents
- •Authors
- •Foreword
- •Acknowledgments
- •Introduction
- •Selection of frameworks
- •Description and evaluation of individual frameworks
- •How to use this handbook
- •Overview of what follows
- •Chapter 1 The nature of thinking and thinking skills
- •Chapter 2 Lists, inventories, groups, taxonomies and frameworks
- •Chapter 3 Frameworks dealing with instructional design
- •Chapter 4 Frameworks dealing with productive thinking
- •Chapter 5 Frameworks dealing with cognitive structure and/or development
- •Chapter 6 Seven ‘all-embracing’ frameworks
- •Chapter 7 Moving from understanding to productive thinking: implications for practice
- •Perspectives on thinking
- •What is thinking?
- •Metacognition and self-regulation
- •Psychological perspectives
- •Sociological perspectives
- •Philosophical perspectives
- •Descriptive or normative?
- •Thinking skills and critical thinking
- •Thinking skills in education
- •Teaching thinking: programmes and approaches
- •Developments in instructional design
- •Bringing order to chaos
- •Objects of study
- •Frameworks
- •Lists
- •Groups
- •Taxonomies
- •Utility
- •Taxonomies and models
- •Maps, charts and diagrams
- •Examples
- •Bloom’s taxonomy
- •Guilford’s structure of intellect model
- •Gerlach and Sullivan’s taxonomy
- •Conclusion
- •Introduction
- •Time sequence of the instructional design frameworks
- •Bloom’s taxonomy of educational objectives (cognitive domain) (1956)
- •Feuerstein’s theory of mediated learning through Instrumental Enrichment (1957)
- •Ausubel and Robinson’s six hierarchically-ordered categories (1969)
- •Williams’ model for developing thinking and feeling processes (1970)
- •Hannah and Michaelis’ comprehensive framework for instructional objectives (1977)
- •Stahl and Murphy’s domain of cognition taxonomic system (1981)
- •Biggs and Collis’ SOLO taxonomy (1982)
- •Quellmalz’s framework of thinking skills (1987)
- •Presseisen’s models of essential, complex and metacognitive thinking skills (1991)
- •Merrill’s instructional transaction theory (1992)
- •Anderson and Krathwohl’s revision of Bloom’s taxonomy (2001)
- •Gouge and Yates’ Arts Project taxonomies of arts reasoning and thinking skills (2002)
- •Description and evaluation of the instructional design frameworks
- •Bloom’s taxonomy of educational objectives: cognitive domain
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Description and intended use
- •Intellectual skills
- •Cognitive strategies
- •Motor skills
- •Attitudes
- •Evaluation
- •Ausubel and Robinson’s six hierarchically-ordered categories
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Williams’ model for developing thinking and feeling processes
- •Description and intended use
- •Cognitive behaviours
- •Affective behaviours
- •Evaluation
- •Hannah and Michaelis’ comprehensive framework for instructional objectives
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Stahl and Murphy’s domain of cognition taxonomic system
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Biggs and Collis’ SOLO taxonomy: Structure of the Observed Learning Outcome
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Quellmalz’s framework of thinking skills
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Presseisen’s models of essential, complex and metacognitive thinking skills
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Merrill’s instructional transaction theory
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Anderson and Krathwohl’s revision of Bloom’s taxonomy of educational objectives
- •Description and intended use
- •Changes in emphasis
- •Changes in terminology
- •Changes in structure
- •Evaluation
- •Gouge and Yates’ ARTS Project taxonomies of arts reasoning and thinking skills
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Some issues for further investigation
- •Introduction
- •Time sequence of the productive-thinking frameworks
- •Altshuller’s TRIZ Theory of Inventive Problem Solving (1956)
- •Allen, Feezel and Kauffie’s taxonomy of critical abilities related to the evaluation of verbal arguments (1967)
- •De Bono’s lateral and parallel thinking tools (1976 / 85)
- •Halpern’s reviews of critical thinking skills and dispositions (1984)
- •Baron’s model of the good thinker (1985)
- •Ennis’ taxonomy of critical thinking dispositions and abilities (1987)
- •Lipman’s modes of thinking and four main varieties of cognitive skill (1991/95)
- •Paul’s model of critical thinking (1993)
- •Jewell’s reasoning taxonomy for gifted children (1996)
- •Petty’s six-phase model of the creative process (1997)
- •Bailin’s intellectual resources for critical thinking (1999b)
- •Description and evaluation of productive-thinking frameworks
- •Description and intended use
- •Problem Definition: in which the would-be solver comes to an understanding of the problem
- •Selecting a Problem-Solving Tool
- •Generating solutions: using the tools
- •Solution evaluation
- •Evaluation
- •Allen, Feezel and Kauffie’s taxonomy of concepts and critical abilities related to the evaluation of verbal arguments
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •De Bono’s lateral and parallel thinking tools
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Halpern’s reviews of critical thinking skills and dispositions
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Baron’s model of the good thinker
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Ennis’ taxonomy of critical thinking dispositions and abilities
- •Description and intended use
- •Dispositions
- •Abilities
- •Clarify
- •Judge the basis for a decision
- •Infer
- •Make suppositions and integrate abilities
- •Use auxiliary critical thinking abilities
- •Evaluation
- •Lipman’s three modes of thinking and four main varieties of cognitive skill
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Paul’s model of critical thinking
- •Description and intended use
- •Elements of reasoning
- •Standards of critical thinking
- •Intellectual abilities
- •Intellectual traits
- •Evaluation
- •Jewell’s reasoning taxonomy for gifted children
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Petty’s six-phase model of the creative process
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Bailin’s intellectual resources for critical thinking
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Some issues for further investigation
- •Introduction
- •Time sequence of theoretical frameworks of cognitive structure and/or development
- •Piaget’s stage model of cognitive development (1950)
- •Guilford’s Structure of Intellect model (1956)
- •Perry’s developmental scheme (1968)
- •Gardner’s theory of multiple intelligences (1983)
- •Koplowitz’s theory of adult cognitive development (1984)
- •Belenky’s ‘Women’s Ways of Knowing’ developmental model (1986)
- •Carroll’s three-stratum theory of cognitive abilities (1993)
- •Demetriou’s integrated developmental model of the mind (1993)
- •King and Kitchener’s model of reflective judgment (1994)
- •Pintrich’s general framework for self-regulated learning (2000)
- •Theories of executive function
- •Description and evaluation of theoretical frameworks of cognitive structure and/or development
- •Piaget’s stage model of cognitive development
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Guilford’s Structure of Intellect model
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Perry’s developmental scheme
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Gardner’s theory of multiple intelligences
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Koplowitz’s theory of adult cognitive development
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Belenky’s ‘Women’s Ways of Knowing’ developmental model
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Carroll’s three-stratum theory of cognitive abilities
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Demetriou’s integrated developmental model of the mind
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •King and Kitchener’s model of reflective judgment
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Pintrich’s general framework for self-regulated learning
- •Description and intended use
- •Regulation of cognition
- •Cognitive planning and activation
- •Cognitive monitoring
- •Cognitive control and regulation
- •Cognitive reaction and reflection
- •Regulation of motivation and affect
- •Motivational planning and activation
- •Motivational monitoring
- •Motivational control and regulation
- •Motivational reaction and reflection
- •Regulation of behaviour
- •Behavioural forethought, planning and action
- •Behavioural monitoring and awareness
- •Behavioural control and regulation
- •Behavioural reaction and reflection
- •Regulation of context
- •Contextual forethought, planning and activation
- •Contextual monitoring
- •Contextual control and regulation
- •Contextual reaction and reflection
- •Evaluation
- •Theories of executive function
- •Description and potential relevance for education
- •Evaluation
- •Some issues for further investigation
- •6 Seven ‘all-embracing’ frameworks
- •Introduction
- •Time sequence of the all-embracing frameworks
- •Romiszowski’s analysis of knowledge and skills (1981)
- •Wallace and Adams’‘ Thinking Actively in a Social Context’ model (1990)
- •Jonassen and Tessmer’s taxonomy of learning outcomes (1996/7)
- •Hauenstein’s conceptual framework for educational objectives (1998)
- •Vermunt and Verloop’s categorisation of learning activities (1999)
- •Marzano’s new taxonomy of educational objectives (2001a; 2001b)
- •Sternberg’s model of abilities as developing expertise (2001)
- •Description and evaluation of seven all-embracing frameworks
- •Romiszowski’s analysis of knowledge and skills
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Jonassen and Tessmer’s taxonomy of learning outcomes
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Hauenstein’s conceptual framework for educational objectives
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Vermunt and Verloop’s categorisation of learning activities
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Marzano’s new taxonomy of educational objectives
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Sternberg’s model of abilities as developing expertise
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Some issues for further investigation
- •Overview
- •How are thinking skills classified?
- •Domain
- •Content
- •Process
- •Psychological aspects
- •Using thinking skills frameworks
- •Which frameworks are best suited to specific applications?
- •Developing appropriate pedagogies
- •Other applications of the frameworks and models
- •In which areas is there extensive or widely accepted knowledge?
- •In which areas is knowledge very limited or highly contested?
- •Constructing an integrated framework
- •Summary
- •References
- •Index
Seven all-embracing frameworks |
261 |
|
|
Table 6.3. Selected tools for effective thinking, using the TASC framework
Gather / organise |
• systematic exploration, using senses and memory |
|
• question available data |
|
• problem recognition |
Identify |
• search for additional information |
|
• explore goals |
|
• question – what is needed? |
|
• represent information clearly |
Generate |
• produce ideas |
|
• consult with others |
|
• compare options |
Decide |
• look at possible consequences |
|
• other people’s views for and against |
|
• establish priorities |
|
• select a course of action |
|
• make a case for the chosen course of action |
|
• plan steps and ways of monitoring |
Implement |
• monitor progress and check efficiency |
|
• consider alternatives and revise plan if necessary |
Evaluate |
• how far goals have been achieved |
|
• efficiency of personal and group processes and |
|
strategies |
Communicate |
• justify decisions |
|
• evaluate the evidence that informed decisions |
|
• exchange ideas on interaction and group organisation |
|
• recall, recount and explain succinctly |
Learn from experience • analyse and reflect on the problem-solving process
•compare present with past performances
•revise the whole problem-solving procedure
•seek to generalise and transfer what has been learned
Evaluation
TASC offers a practical framework to support problem-solving through the structure of its organising ‘wheel’ or stages of the process.
262 Frameworks for Thinking
The broad categories of thinking constitute a framework rather than a taxonomy, as a number of the skills in the different sections overlap. The TASC model includes all the cognitive and metacognitive categories one might expect, including strategic thinking and reflection on what has been learned. The TASC ‘Tools for Effective Thinking’ comprise a mixture of strategies that accommodate logical, creative and practical thinking. Dispositions are included under attitudinal and motivational factors, as well as under the communication heading. The framework is offered as a guide to structure and develop thinking, especially through collaborative problem-solving and by enabling parents, teachers and learners to break each of the stages down into manageable skill areas where this is needed.
Table 6.4. Elaborated descriptions of selected TASC skill areas
Attitudinal and • being purposeful and optimistic motivational factors • interacting actively with the environment
•avoiding impulsive responses
•recognising the need for systematic exploration, accuracy and precision, making comparisons, summarising experiences, planning, being flexible in approaching problems, being persistent
•being willing to work co-operatively or independently as the occasion demands
Metacognition • being aware of incongruity, incompletion, the existence of a problem
•selecting appropriate modes of problem representation
•selecting cognitive strategies
•allocating attentional resources
•solution monitoring
•sensitivity to feedback
•awareness of one’s strengths and weaknesses, and acting accordingly
•balance between critical, analytical and creative thinking
•planning
Seven all-embracing frameworks |
263 |
|
|
Using gathered |
• relating new data to previous experiences |
information to |
• being aware of disequilibrium, incompletion, |
identify and solve |
incongruity |
problems |
• distinguishing between relevant and irrelevant |
|
information |
|
• selecting of representation: e.g. codes, conventions, |
|
symbols, diagrams, pictures, drawings, tables, |
|
charts, summaries, keywords, spider diagrams |
|
• seeking relationships between objects, events, |
|
experiences |
|
• keeping in mind various pieces of information |
|
• comparing objects, events, experiences |
|
• finding the class or set to which objects, events, |
|
experiences belong |
|
• understanding and using spatial and temporal |
|
references and patterns, including various viewpoints |
|
• analysing information, problems into parts |
|
• synthesising ideas from various sources |
|
• thinking about different possibilities and |
|
consequences |
|
• using logical evidence to prove things and |
|
defend opinions |
Communicating • avoiding egocentric communication –
with co-learners thinking things through before beginning to and communicating communicate
the outcome • being clear and precise – avoiding blocking
•selecting an appropriate mode for communication
•giving instructions clearly – using logical evidence to defend opinions
•being an active listener
Learning from • comparing new experiences with previous ones experience • classifying objects, events, experiences, problems,
solutions
•considering other circumstances in which the information, experience, outcome, insight might apply
•deriving rules and principles from experiences
•hypothesising and predicting about related problems/issues
264 Frameworks for Thinking
The importance of motivation is appropriately stressed by Wallace and Adams, and in the books which support TASC there are many examples of problem-solving which learners will find emotionally engaging.
As with Halpern’s approach to teaching critical thinking (Halpern, 2002), the TASC framework is intended to help with the development of lifeskills (Wallace, 2003). To this end it encourages discussion and dialogue, as well as the public sharing of thinking after the event. The emphasis on addressing real-life problems is part of the authors’ attempt to facilitate the transfer and future use of problem-solving skills.
The TASC approach draws on a rationale based on the work of Vygotsky (1978) and the importance of social interaction in developing higher psychological processes. Other information-processing theories of intelligence and cognitive development are also cited, in particular Sternberg’s (1985) ‘Triarchic Theory of Intelligence’, and Borkowski’s model of the executive system (Borkowski, 1985). Metacognition is treated so as to bring out its knowledge and working aspects, and the descriptors used convey the importance of cognitive self-regulation through what cognitive psychologists call executive function. A simpler way of describing TASC is that it is not only about problem-solving, it is about learning how to learn.
As to the detail included within each sector or area of the model, the authors make no claim for comprehensive coverage. The extended analysis was developed after early classroom trials had indicated that a more specific focus was needed in certain areas. The language used is not intended to be prescriptive, but it is meant to be shared, with alternative simpler phrasing negotiated with learners where needed. This pragmatic approach to framework development is a highly distinctive feature of TASC.
Teachers will find the TASC cycle easy to understand and will welcome the fact that they can adapt it to the needs of pupils in different areas of the curriculum. It does, however, present significant challenges in that teachers are expected to model the processes they wish to develop, to provide frequent opportunities for learners to practise problem-solving and to move learners towards much greater autonomy.
Seven all-embracing frameworks |
265 |
|
|
Some evaluation of the impact of the TASC approach has been undertaken: on problem-solving (Maltby, 1995), on gifted pupils in South Africa (van der Horst, 2000) and as a means of staff development (Adams and Wallace, 1991).
Summary: Wallace and Adams
|
|
|
|
Relevance for teachers |
|
Purpose and structure |
Some key features |
and learning |
|||
|
|
|
|||
Main purpose(s): |
Terminology: |
Intended audience: |
|||
• |
to support the |
• |
very clear terminology |
• |
curriculum developers |
|
development of |
|
in everyday language, |
• |
educational |
|
problem-solving and |
|
to be used by learners |
|
psychologists |
|
thereby improve |
|
in discussion and |
• |
teachers and parents |
|
achievement and |
|
reflection |
• |
learners, including |
|
attitudes |
|
|
|
those for whom English |
• |
to prepare students for |
|
|
|
is a second language |
|
active roles in society |
|
|
|
|
Domains addressed: |
Presentation: |
Contexts: |
|||
• |
cognitive |
• |
as a series of practical |
• |
education |
• |
affective |
|
guides for teachers |
• |
work |
• |
conative |
|
with the theoretical |
• |
citizenship |
• |
social |
|
rationale developed |
• |
recreation |
|
|
|
in articles |
|
|
Broad categories |
Theory base: |
Pedagogical stance: |
|||
covered: |
• |
Vygotsky’s |
• |
start with real-life |
|
• |
self engagement |
|
development of |
|
problems |
|
and self-regulation |
|
higher psychological |
• |
collaborative |
• |
reflective thinking |
|
processes |
|
problem-solving as a |
• |
productive thinking |
• |
Sternberg’s ‘Triarchic |
|
practical context in |
• |
building understanding |
|
Theory of Intelligence’ |
|
which to develop |
• |
information-gathering |
• |
Bandura’s social |
|
transferable skills |
|
|
|
learning theory |
• |
move from modelling to |
|
|
|
|
|
guided activity to |
|
|
|
|
|
autonomy |
|
|
|
|
• |
provide ample practice |
|
|
|
|
|
in strategy use |
|
|
|
|
• |
emphasise motivation |
|
|
|
|
|
and self-regulation |