- •Contents
- •Authors
- •Foreword
- •Acknowledgments
- •Introduction
- •Selection of frameworks
- •Description and evaluation of individual frameworks
- •How to use this handbook
- •Overview of what follows
- •Chapter 1 The nature of thinking and thinking skills
- •Chapter 2 Lists, inventories, groups, taxonomies and frameworks
- •Chapter 3 Frameworks dealing with instructional design
- •Chapter 4 Frameworks dealing with productive thinking
- •Chapter 5 Frameworks dealing with cognitive structure and/or development
- •Chapter 6 Seven ‘all-embracing’ frameworks
- •Chapter 7 Moving from understanding to productive thinking: implications for practice
- •Perspectives on thinking
- •What is thinking?
- •Metacognition and self-regulation
- •Psychological perspectives
- •Sociological perspectives
- •Philosophical perspectives
- •Descriptive or normative?
- •Thinking skills and critical thinking
- •Thinking skills in education
- •Teaching thinking: programmes and approaches
- •Developments in instructional design
- •Bringing order to chaos
- •Objects of study
- •Frameworks
- •Lists
- •Groups
- •Taxonomies
- •Utility
- •Taxonomies and models
- •Maps, charts and diagrams
- •Examples
- •Bloom’s taxonomy
- •Guilford’s structure of intellect model
- •Gerlach and Sullivan’s taxonomy
- •Conclusion
- •Introduction
- •Time sequence of the instructional design frameworks
- •Bloom’s taxonomy of educational objectives (cognitive domain) (1956)
- •Feuerstein’s theory of mediated learning through Instrumental Enrichment (1957)
- •Ausubel and Robinson’s six hierarchically-ordered categories (1969)
- •Williams’ model for developing thinking and feeling processes (1970)
- •Hannah and Michaelis’ comprehensive framework for instructional objectives (1977)
- •Stahl and Murphy’s domain of cognition taxonomic system (1981)
- •Biggs and Collis’ SOLO taxonomy (1982)
- •Quellmalz’s framework of thinking skills (1987)
- •Presseisen’s models of essential, complex and metacognitive thinking skills (1991)
- •Merrill’s instructional transaction theory (1992)
- •Anderson and Krathwohl’s revision of Bloom’s taxonomy (2001)
- •Gouge and Yates’ Arts Project taxonomies of arts reasoning and thinking skills (2002)
- •Description and evaluation of the instructional design frameworks
- •Bloom’s taxonomy of educational objectives: cognitive domain
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Description and intended use
- •Intellectual skills
- •Cognitive strategies
- •Motor skills
- •Attitudes
- •Evaluation
- •Ausubel and Robinson’s six hierarchically-ordered categories
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Williams’ model for developing thinking and feeling processes
- •Description and intended use
- •Cognitive behaviours
- •Affective behaviours
- •Evaluation
- •Hannah and Michaelis’ comprehensive framework for instructional objectives
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Stahl and Murphy’s domain of cognition taxonomic system
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Biggs and Collis’ SOLO taxonomy: Structure of the Observed Learning Outcome
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Quellmalz’s framework of thinking skills
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Presseisen’s models of essential, complex and metacognitive thinking skills
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Merrill’s instructional transaction theory
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Anderson and Krathwohl’s revision of Bloom’s taxonomy of educational objectives
- •Description and intended use
- •Changes in emphasis
- •Changes in terminology
- •Changes in structure
- •Evaluation
- •Gouge and Yates’ ARTS Project taxonomies of arts reasoning and thinking skills
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Some issues for further investigation
- •Introduction
- •Time sequence of the productive-thinking frameworks
- •Altshuller’s TRIZ Theory of Inventive Problem Solving (1956)
- •Allen, Feezel and Kauffie’s taxonomy of critical abilities related to the evaluation of verbal arguments (1967)
- •De Bono’s lateral and parallel thinking tools (1976 / 85)
- •Halpern’s reviews of critical thinking skills and dispositions (1984)
- •Baron’s model of the good thinker (1985)
- •Ennis’ taxonomy of critical thinking dispositions and abilities (1987)
- •Lipman’s modes of thinking and four main varieties of cognitive skill (1991/95)
- •Paul’s model of critical thinking (1993)
- •Jewell’s reasoning taxonomy for gifted children (1996)
- •Petty’s six-phase model of the creative process (1997)
- •Bailin’s intellectual resources for critical thinking (1999b)
- •Description and evaluation of productive-thinking frameworks
- •Description and intended use
- •Problem Definition: in which the would-be solver comes to an understanding of the problem
- •Selecting a Problem-Solving Tool
- •Generating solutions: using the tools
- •Solution evaluation
- •Evaluation
- •Allen, Feezel and Kauffie’s taxonomy of concepts and critical abilities related to the evaluation of verbal arguments
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •De Bono’s lateral and parallel thinking tools
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Halpern’s reviews of critical thinking skills and dispositions
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Baron’s model of the good thinker
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Ennis’ taxonomy of critical thinking dispositions and abilities
- •Description and intended use
- •Dispositions
- •Abilities
- •Clarify
- •Judge the basis for a decision
- •Infer
- •Make suppositions and integrate abilities
- •Use auxiliary critical thinking abilities
- •Evaluation
- •Lipman’s three modes of thinking and four main varieties of cognitive skill
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Paul’s model of critical thinking
- •Description and intended use
- •Elements of reasoning
- •Standards of critical thinking
- •Intellectual abilities
- •Intellectual traits
- •Evaluation
- •Jewell’s reasoning taxonomy for gifted children
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Petty’s six-phase model of the creative process
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Bailin’s intellectual resources for critical thinking
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Some issues for further investigation
- •Introduction
- •Time sequence of theoretical frameworks of cognitive structure and/or development
- •Piaget’s stage model of cognitive development (1950)
- •Guilford’s Structure of Intellect model (1956)
- •Perry’s developmental scheme (1968)
- •Gardner’s theory of multiple intelligences (1983)
- •Koplowitz’s theory of adult cognitive development (1984)
- •Belenky’s ‘Women’s Ways of Knowing’ developmental model (1986)
- •Carroll’s three-stratum theory of cognitive abilities (1993)
- •Demetriou’s integrated developmental model of the mind (1993)
- •King and Kitchener’s model of reflective judgment (1994)
- •Pintrich’s general framework for self-regulated learning (2000)
- •Theories of executive function
- •Description and evaluation of theoretical frameworks of cognitive structure and/or development
- •Piaget’s stage model of cognitive development
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Guilford’s Structure of Intellect model
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Perry’s developmental scheme
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Gardner’s theory of multiple intelligences
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Koplowitz’s theory of adult cognitive development
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Belenky’s ‘Women’s Ways of Knowing’ developmental model
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Carroll’s three-stratum theory of cognitive abilities
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Demetriou’s integrated developmental model of the mind
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •King and Kitchener’s model of reflective judgment
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Pintrich’s general framework for self-regulated learning
- •Description and intended use
- •Regulation of cognition
- •Cognitive planning and activation
- •Cognitive monitoring
- •Cognitive control and regulation
- •Cognitive reaction and reflection
- •Regulation of motivation and affect
- •Motivational planning and activation
- •Motivational monitoring
- •Motivational control and regulation
- •Motivational reaction and reflection
- •Regulation of behaviour
- •Behavioural forethought, planning and action
- •Behavioural monitoring and awareness
- •Behavioural control and regulation
- •Behavioural reaction and reflection
- •Regulation of context
- •Contextual forethought, planning and activation
- •Contextual monitoring
- •Contextual control and regulation
- •Contextual reaction and reflection
- •Evaluation
- •Theories of executive function
- •Description and potential relevance for education
- •Evaluation
- •Some issues for further investigation
- •6 Seven ‘all-embracing’ frameworks
- •Introduction
- •Time sequence of the all-embracing frameworks
- •Romiszowski’s analysis of knowledge and skills (1981)
- •Wallace and Adams’‘ Thinking Actively in a Social Context’ model (1990)
- •Jonassen and Tessmer’s taxonomy of learning outcomes (1996/7)
- •Hauenstein’s conceptual framework for educational objectives (1998)
- •Vermunt and Verloop’s categorisation of learning activities (1999)
- •Marzano’s new taxonomy of educational objectives (2001a; 2001b)
- •Sternberg’s model of abilities as developing expertise (2001)
- •Description and evaluation of seven all-embracing frameworks
- •Romiszowski’s analysis of knowledge and skills
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Jonassen and Tessmer’s taxonomy of learning outcomes
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Hauenstein’s conceptual framework for educational objectives
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Vermunt and Verloop’s categorisation of learning activities
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Marzano’s new taxonomy of educational objectives
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Sternberg’s model of abilities as developing expertise
- •Description and intended use
- •Evaluation
- •Some issues for further investigation
- •Overview
- •How are thinking skills classified?
- •Domain
- •Content
- •Process
- •Psychological aspects
- •Using thinking skills frameworks
- •Which frameworks are best suited to specific applications?
- •Developing appropriate pedagogies
- •Other applications of the frameworks and models
- •In which areas is there extensive or widely accepted knowledge?
- •In which areas is knowledge very limited or highly contested?
- •Constructing an integrated framework
- •Summary
- •References
- •Index
112 Frameworks for Thinking
Summary: Anderson and Krathwohl
|
|
|
|
Relevance for |
|
Purpose and structure |
Some key features |
teachers and learning |
|||
|
|
|
|||
Main purpose(s): |
Terminology: |
Intended audience: |
|||
• |
to support the delivery |
• |
clear definitions |
• |
teachers |
|
of a standards-based |
|
|
• |
curriculum planners |
|
curriculum through |
|
|
|
|
|
the use of a revision |
|
|
|
|
|
of Bloom’s taxonomy |
|
|
|
|
Domains addressed: |
Presentation: |
Contexts: |
|||
• |
cognitive |
• |
accessible to |
• |
education |
|
|
|
a wide readership |
|
|
|
|
• |
good use of tables |
|
|
|
|
|
and matrices |
|
|
Broad categories covered: |
Theory base: |
Pedagogical stance: |
|||
• |
reflective thinking |
• |
not based |
• |
belief in improving |
• |
productive thinking |
|
on a single |
|
cognitive performance |
• |
building understanding |
|
psychological |
|
through the alignment |
• |
information-gathering |
|
theory, but |
|
of learning objectives, |
|
|
|
compatible with |
|
assessment and |
|
|
|
many |
|
instruction |
Classification by: |
Values: |
Practical illustrations |
|||
• |
cognitive processes |
• |
neutral, except |
for teachers: |
|
|
and types of knowledge |
|
for favouring |
• |
a series of vignettes |
|
|
|
higher-level thinking |
|
illustrates key concepts |
|
|
|
|
|
and elements of the |
|
|
|
|
|
taxonomy table |
Gouge and Yates’ ARTS Project taxonomies of arts reasoning and thinking skills
Description and intended use
These taxonomies were devised by a cognitive acceleration project team seeking to develop a new approach to the teaching of thinking through the creative arts (visual arts, music and drama). The theories
Instructional design |
113 |
|
|
informing this approach are those of Piaget (1950) and Vygotsky (1978) [which also underpin the well-known CASE and CAME cognitive acceleration programmes in science (Adey, Shayer and Yates 1995) and mathematics (Adhami, Johnson and Shayer, 1998)]. Gouge and Yates (2002, p. 137) describe how three taxonomies were devised, using basically the same framework ‘in order to provide a consistent structure for designing a programme of intervention lessons’ for pupils aged 11–14.
In essence, Gouge and Yates have produced a framework for classifying the reasoning skills involved in creative thinking. They state (2002, p. 137) that creativity ‘requires mental discipline, previous experience and a firm grounding in knowledge’, and see dangers in the notion that the arts are all about ‘fun’ and free expression.
Three Piagetian levels of cognitive demand are used: concrete, concrete transitional and formal operational thinking. These are said to correspond with Peel’s (1971) restricted, circumstantial, and imaginative comprehensive stages of adolescent judgement. Although ‘formal operation thought can begin to develop at about the age of 12’, Gouge and Yates claim (2002, p. 137) that even by the age of 16, few adolescents are ‘deductive, rational and systematic’ in their thinking, able to ‘reason about hypothetical events that are not necessarily in accord with their direct experience’. Their aim is to accelerate adolescent cognitive development beyond the level where pupils can only ‘make simple assumptions and deductions to offer imaginative explanations’.
Five reasoning patterns are common to all three taxonomies, but a sixth pattern (narrative seriation) is used in the taxonomy for drama. The common 3 5 matrix, within which sets of educational objectives are located, is illustrated in table 3.10.
The six reasoning patterns are based on unpublished work by Fusco (1983). They are not ordered by any principle and no claim is made as to their comprehensiveness. A summary is provided below:
•classification – the ability to group or order attributes or objects by one attribute or criterion
•frames of reference – dealing with relativity of thought by attempting to reconcile conflicting information and reach closure
114Frameworks for Thinking
•symbolic reasoning –the use of a wide range of visual and auditory symbols to create imagery and perspective and to communicate ideas
•critical reflection – the development of judgment, from restricted to imaginative and comprehensive forms
•intention, causality and experimentation – the act of making, including hypothesising and trialling
•narrative seriation – the ability to sequence and re-sequence actions to create a narrative and to manipulate components to give multiple meanings and layers of complexity.
Each cell in the taxonomy framework contains between two and four educational objectives. The distinctions between the Piagetian stage levels are expressed in several ways, including the number of variables or viewpoints involved, the level of abstraction and the use of argument to support diverse interpretations. Here is an illustrative example for the reasoning pattern classification, taken from the taxonomy for music:
Concrete: identify similarities and differences in music; for example, mood and pace.
Concrete transitional: compare and contrast pieces of music using more than two variables simultaneously.
Formal operational: make rich comparisons of two or more pieces of music, identifying multiple variables such as context, style and instrumentation.
Gouge and Yates do not move beyond a Piagetian framework into a conception of ‘post-formal’ or ‘post-logical’ thought, although they do acknowledge that it is not always possible to arrive at firm conclusions on artistic matters. The overall impression is that they have tried to bring an analytic scientific perspective to bear on the creative arts in ‘an attempt to deconstruct the neglected aspects of critical thinking which practising artists use intuitively, and which they usually have difficulty in articulating’ (2002, p. 138).
Cognitive acceleration is based on the five pedagogical principles, with reasoning patterns being the focus of each lesson. These principles are as follows:
Instructional design |
115 |
|
|
Table 3.10. The common framework used in the ARTS reasoning taxonomies
|
|
|
|
Intention, |
|
|
|
|
causality |
|
Frames |
|
|
and |
Classifi- |
of |
Symbolic |
Critical |
experi- |
cation |
reference |
reasoning |
reflection |
mentation |
Concrete
Concrete
transitional
Formal operational thinking
1.cognitive conflict within Vygotsky’s ‘zone of proximal development’
2.social construction of knowledge with teacher and peer mediation
3.preparation (including establishing a shared language) and ‘bridging’ (creating links to facilitate transfer to other domains of experience)
4.metacognition (thinking about one’s own thinking)
5.reasoning patterns (in this case, the six patterns listed above).
Evaluation
Gouge and Yates have extended to the creative arts an established cognitive acceleration approach which was first developed for science and mathematics teaching. This appears to give pride of place to an analytic reasoned approach and the ability to handle complex abstractions in an area where other values (such as emphasising concrete experience and emotional resonance) often prevail. At the same time they include types of thinking and expression which are well established in the creative arts, notably imagery and narrative. Their initiative is intended to be a challenge to teachers ‘to restructure their attitudes and behaviour as mediators of cognitive development’ (2002, p. 138).
Some will undoubtedly welcome this approach, seeing it as providing a respectable and rigorous academic framework which can be used
116 Frameworks for Thinking
to defend the creative arts against attack by philistines. Others will feel that qualitatively different kinds of thinking are important in artistic expression and that formal operation thinking is too closely allied to traditional conceptions of working memory and intelligence. Some may see it as being inimical to creativity.
It remains to be seen how teachers will respond to cognitive acceleration in the arts. It lays down a challenge to those who believe that spontaneity is all and knowledge and skills relatively unimportant. It may well stimulate theorists and practitioners to achieve a new synthesis between the affective, motivational and cognitive aspects of their practice.
Summary: Gouge and Yates
|
|
|
|
Relevance for |
|
Purpose and structure |
Some key features |
teachers and learning |
|||
|
|
Intended audience: |
|||
Main purpose(s): |
Terminology: |
||||
• |
to use the Arts |
• |
clear |
• |
teachers |
|
as a vehicle for |
• |
technical terms |
|
|
|
Cognitive |
|
are explained |
|
|
|
Acceleration |
|
|
|
|
• |
to promote creative |
|
|
|
|
|
and critical thinking |
|
|
|
|
Domains addressed: |
Presentation: |
Contexts: |
|||
• |
cognitive |
• |
one-page tabular |
• |
education |
• |
conative |
|
format |
|
|
|
|
• |
well-structured |
|
|
|
|
|
and not too |
|
|
|
|
|
complex |
|
|
Broad categories covered: |
Theory base: |
Pedagogical stance: |
|||
• |
reflective thinking |
• |
Piaget’s genetic |
• |
directive, but also |
• |
productive thinking |
|
epistemology |
|
facilitatory in enabling |
• |
building understanding |
• |
Vygotsky |
|
the mediation and |
• |
information-gathering |
|
|
|
construction of |
|
|
|
|
|
meaning |
|
|
|
|
• |
learning through |
|
|
|
|
|
peer-coaching and |
|
|
|
|
|
collaboration |