
- •1) Homework 11-33 (up to Historical (diachronic) linguistics): read and refresh your knowledge, be ready to answer questions on the subject
- •Make a short report or presentation about one of the scientist from the part “History of linguistics” (p. 2-11)
- •Greek and Roman antiquity
- •The European Middle Ages
- •Other 19th-century theories and development
- •Inner and outer form
- •Phonetics and dialectology
- •The 20th century Structuralism
- •Structural linguistics in Europe
- •Structural linguistics in America
- •Transformational-generative grammar
- •Methods of synchronic linguistic analysis Structural linguistics
- •Phonology
- •Morphology
- •Semantics
- •Transformational-generative grammar
- •Harris’s grammar
- •Modifications in Chomsky’s grammar
- •Tagmemics
- •Modes of language
- •Hierarchy of levels
- •Stratificational grammar
- •Technical terminology
- •Interstratal relationships
- •The Prague school
- •Combination of structuralism and functionalism
- •Phonological contributions
- •Theory of markedness
- •Later contributions
- •Historical (diachronic) linguistics Linguistic change
- •Sound change
- •Grammatical change
- •Semantic change
- •Borrowing
- •The comparative method
- •Steps in the comparative method
- •Criticisms of the comparative method
- •Internal reconstruction
- •Language classification
- •Linguistics and other disciplines Psycholinguistics
- •Language acquisition by children
- •Speech perception
- •Other areas of research
- •Sociolinguistics Delineation of the field
- •Social dimensions
- •Other relationships Anthropological linguistics
- •Early dialect studies
- •Dialect atlases
- •The value and applications of dialectology
Criticisms of the comparative method
One
of the criticisms directed against the comparative method is that it
is based upon a misleading genealogical metaphor. In the mid-19th
century, the German linguist August Schleicher introduced into
comparative linguistics the model of the “family tree.” There is
obviously no point in time at which it can be said that new languages
are “born” of a common parent language. Nor is it normally the
case that the parent language “lives on” for a while, relatively
unchanged, and then “dies.” It is easy enough to recognize the
inappropriateness of these biological expressions. No less
misleading, however, is the assumption that languages descended from
the same parent language will necessarily diverge, never to converge
again, through time. This assumption is built into the comparative
method as it is traditionally applied. And yet there are many clear
cases of convergence in the development of well-documented languages.
The dialects
of England are fast disappearing and are far more similar in grammar
and vocabulary today than they were even a generation ago. They have
been strongly influenced by the standard language. The same
phenomenon, the replacement of nonstandard or less prestigious forms
with forms borrowed from the standard language or dialect, has taken
place in many different places at many different times. It would
seem, therefore, that one must reckon with both divergence and
convergence in the diachronic development of languages: divergence
when contact between two speech communities is reduced or broken and
convergence when the two speech communities remain in contact and
when one is politically or culturally dominant.
The comparative method presupposes linguistically uniform speech communities and independent development after sudden, sharp cleavage. Critics of the comparative method have pointed out that this situation does not generally hold. In 1872 a German scholar, Johannes Schmidt, criticized the family-tree theory and proposed instead what is referred to as the wave theory, according to which different linguistic changes will spread, like waves, from a politically, commercially, or culturally important centre along the main lines of communication, but successive innovations will not necessarily cover exactly the same area. Consequently, there will be no sharp distinction between contiguous dialects, but, in general, the further apart two speech communities are, the more linguistic features there will be that distinguish them.
Internal reconstruction
The comparative method is used to reconstruct earlier forms of a language by drawing upon the evidence provided by other related languages. It may be supplemented by what is called the method of internal reconstruction. This is based upon the existence of anomalous or irregular patterns of formation and the assumption that they must have developed, usually by sound change, from earlier regular patterns. For example, the existence of such patterns in early Latin as honos : honoris (“honor” : “of honor”) and others in contrast with orator : oratoris (“orator” : “of the orator”) and others might lead to the supposition that honoris developed from an earlier *honosis. In this case, the evidence of other languages shows that *s became r between vowels in an earlier period of Latin. But it would have been possible to reconstruct the earlier intervocalic *s with a fair degree of confidence on the basis of the internal evidence alone. Clearly, internal reconstruction depends upon the structural approach to linguistics.
The most significant 20th-century development in the field of historical and comparative linguistics came from the theory of generative grammar. If the grammar and phonology of a language are described from a synchronic point of view as an integrated system of rules, then the grammatical and phonological similarities and differences between two closely related languages, or dialects, or between two diachronically distinct states of the same language can be described in terms of the similarities and differences in two descriptive rule systems. One system may contain a rule that the other lacks (or may restrict its application more or less narrowly); one system may differ from the other in that the same set of rules will apply in a different order in the one system from the order in which they apply in the other. Language change may thus be accounted for in terms of changes introduced into the underlying system of phonological and grammatical rules (including the addition, loss, or reordering of rules) during the process of language acquisition. So far these principles have been applied principally to sound change. There has also been a little work done on diachronic syntax.