Добавил:
Опубликованный материал нарушает ваши авторские права? Сообщите нам.
Вуз: Предмет: Файл:

boudlal_abdelaziz_the_prosody_and_morphology_of_a_moroccan_a-1

.pdf
Скачиваний:
47
Добавлен:
15.04.2019
Размер:
3.99 Mб
Скачать

Both candidates 63b and 63c incur a fatal violation of higher-ranked IDENT-BR [cons] either because the reduplicant is a high vowel and the base is a glide or vice versa. 63b fails exactly because the reduplicant which is [i] in this case corresponds to [y] in the base; candidate 63c is also excluded because the reduplicant [y] corresponds to the base [i]. The optimal candidate 63a satisfies IDENT-BR [cons] at the expense of low-ranked IDENT-IO [cons]. Here, the input vowel associated to a mora is realized as a glide in the output, thus losing its moraic status. Of a particular interest in tableau 63 is that the base copies the reduplicant, a state of affairs which leads to the violation of input-output faithfulness. Given that the second segment of the input is /i/, its realization as the glide [y] will automatically lead to the violation of IDENT-IO [cons]. Such account is available only under CT where an identity relation holds between the base and the reduplicant, on the one hand, and between the input and the output, on the other.

One may wonder why a form such as *[fiyiq] is ruled out although it satisfies both IDENT-BR [cons] and IDENT-IO [cons] by virtue of the fact that the reduplicant has a correspondent in the base (the glide being epenthesized to satisfy ONSET). It should be noted that the causative form is governed by a prosodic constraint which requires that the output form consist exactly of an iambic foot of the type LL. The constraint LL will have to dominate MAX-Rt-BR so as to exclude cases that resort to total reduplication or any other type of feet as the tableau below shows:

-64-

 

LL

MAX-Rt-BR

 

DEP-IO

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

a. fəy.yəq

 

**

**

 

 

 

 

 

b. fi.yiq

*!

**

*

 

 

 

 

 

c. fiq.fiq

*!

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

d. fi.fiq

*!

*

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

All of the candidates, except 64a, are ruled out because they fail to conform to an iambic foot of the type LL. The optimal candidate has proceeded to a double epenthesis of a schwa to achieve the desired foot type. Note also that this candidate violates IDENT-IO [cons] but this violation is achieved for the purpose of establishing featural correspondence between the base and the reduplicant.

199

Next, consider another causative case obtained from verbs whose second segment is the high vocoid /u/. The tableau below lists some possible candidates from the input /RED, dub/:

-65-

/RED, dub/

ONSET

 

ALIGN-E

L-ANCHOR-

MAX-Rt-BR

 

 

 

(Root, PWd)

BR

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

)a.dəw.wəb

 

 

 

*

**

 

 

 

 

 

 

b. du.ub

*!

 

 

*

**

 

 

 

 

 

 

c. dub.dub

 

***!

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

d. du.bəb

 

 

 

**!

**

 

 

 

 

 

 

Once again, the constraints developed above predict that it is always the form that copies the second segment of the base which is optimal. Of course, this form has to satisfy higherranked constraints such as ONSET and ALIGN-E (Root, PWd). Note that the constraint LL considered in 64 blocks total reduplication and in so doing does part of the functions performed by ALIGN-E (Root, PWd). In the optimal candidate in 65, the input vowel, which is /u/ loses the mora associated with it and surfaces as the glide [w] due to undominated IDENT-BR [cons] requiring identity between the base and the reduplicant.

Finally, let us consider a causative form obtained from verbs whose final segment is a vocoid. From the input /RED, bki/, Gen would allow the generation of output candidates such as the following:

-66-

/RED, bki/

ALIGN-E

L-ANCHOR-BR

MAX-Rt-BR

 

(Root, PWd)

 

 

 

 

 

 

)a. bək.ki

 

*

**

 

 

 

 

b. bəy.ki

 

**!

**

 

 

 

 

c. bək.yi

 

**!

**

 

 

 

 

Both candidate 66b and 66c are excluded because they incur two violations of L-ANCHOR- BR. The optimal candidate itself incurs only a single violation of the same constraint. As to the IDENT constraint family, candidates 66b and 66c violate IDENT-BR [cons] because the reduplicant, which is a glide, stands in correspondence with the base vowel.

200

To conclude, we have argued that the causative in CMA could be accounted for adequately by assuming a set of universal constraints ranked on a language-particular basis. In essence we have shown that operational circumscription is not warranted for the different steps it involves. We have also shown that the reanalysis of circumscription as prosodic faithfulness to some designated syllable edge can neither account for verb bases having the shape /CVC/ nor block long distance consonantal spreading. We have instead proposed an output-output analysis based on the idea that the causative involves partial reduplication of the second segment of the base. The output-output relation dealt with in this section involves two strings produced simultaneously (i.e. the base and the reduplicant) which do not exist as separate words. This relation differs from the output-output relation encountered in section 3 above involving separate words that are not produced simultaneously. (For a comparison between the two output-output relations, the reader is referred to Benua 1995, 1997).

6. CONCLUSION

This chapter has tried to analyze phonological similarities between words that stand in a transderivational relationship. It has been shown that cases originally attributed to cyclicity, truncation and circumscription could be accounted for by output-output constraints demanding correspondence between a derived form and its morphologically related form. The cases considered were cyclic syllabification, truncation and finally causative formation.

In dealing with cyclic syllabification in CMA, we have proposed an analysis in terms of a small subset of constraints, one of which demands faithfulness of the derived form to the simple base form. Our notion of the base rests essentially on the definition set up in Basri et al (1998) and Selkirk (1999). These authors have proposed a morphologically grounded theory of O-O correspondence that distinguishes two different O-O faithfulness constraints: O-Oword Faith and O-Ostem Faith. In order to account for cyclic syllabification in CMA, we have proposed two different O-O constraints formulated after Selkirk (1999) and consistent with the correspondence model of McCarthy and Prince (1995, 1999): O-Ostem ANCHOR (σ, σ, Initial) and O-Oword ANCHOR (σ, σ, Initial). This distinction is based on the distribution of subject and object suffixes. In particular, we have shown that O-Ostem ANCHOR (σ, σ, Initial) must dominate O-Oword ANCHOR (σ, σ, Initial) based on items such as [DRəb-t] which does not epenthesize a schwa before the subject suffix and [DəRbək] which

201

epenthesizes a schwa before the object suffix and hence violates O-Oword ANCHOR (σ, σ, Initial). We have also shown that O-Ostem ANCHOR (σ, σ, Initial) is never violated except when the markedness constraints ONSET and *COMPLEX are at stake. Resorting to a morphologically-grounded theory of O-O correspondence to account for cyclic syllabification obviates the need for any mechanisms, such as those used in rule-based systems, which try to explain cyclicity by reference to intermediate stages in the input-output mapping process.

The chapter has also dealt with cases where part of the base is truncated in the formation of some nisba adjectives derived from compound nouns and from nouns with the affixes [ta-...-t] and [ʔa-]. We have argued that in order to adequately account for these nisba adjectives, recourse needs to be made to morphology, and especially to the distinction between the stem and the word. We have shown that truncation follows from ranking *[N + Adj]PWd along with the affixation constraint AFFIX-TO-Nstem and the alignment constraint ALIGN-Nisba-R above the constraint MORPH-REAL, thus forcing deletion of the leftmost stem of a compound and the nominal affixes [ta-...-t] and [ʔa-] in toponyms.

Finally, following Imouzaz (forthcoming) we have argued that the causative could adequately be accounted for in terms of O-O correspondence relating the base and its reduplicant. We have shown that the constraint ALIGN-E (Root, PWd) proposed in Nelson (1998) is very active in CMA since it prevents total reduplication and forces the reduplicant to be an infix. We have also shown that the causative form of words such as [fəyyəq] “wake up” and [dəwwəb] “dissolve”, derived from the roots /fiq/ and /dub/, exhibits a special behavior in that the base in both forms copies the reduplicant which is [y] in the first item and [w] in the second. Such an explanation could only be achieved if we posit constraints on the output such as the one between the base and the reduplicant.

202

Chapter Five

The Prosodic Nature of

the Passive Participle in Casablanca Moroccan Arabic

1. INTRODUCTION

One of the most productive morphological operations in MA is the derivation of the passive participle (PP). It has received different treatments according to different linguists. The difference resides in the nature of the PP morpheme. Some linguists (see Marsil 1988, for example) assume that the PP is obtained via the affixation of the discontinuous morpheme [m...u...] to the base, which is assumed to be the verb root. [m-] is prefixed to the root while [-u-] is infixed after the second segment of the base. Others (Youssi 1986, and Boudlal 1993) assume that the PP morpheme is simply the morpheme [m-] which is prefixed to the base.

This chapter gives further support to the stress pattern of CMA and consequently the foot types proposed in chapter 3. The idea defended here is that the PP marker is [m-] and that the prefinal vowel that appears in some forms is the lexical default segment /u/ which is epenthesized to conform to an iambic foot of the type LH. It will be shown that the LL foot type arises only in cases where the epenthesis of /u/ does not take place, something that suggests which there are other constraints that outrank those governing foot types. In particular it will be shown that the PP forms that do not end up in a heavy syllable can only be obtained by making recourse to constraints on output-output correspondence much in the spirit undertaken in chapter four.

The chapter is divided into three major sections. In section 2, a taxonomic survey of the PP forms is offered. The verb bases considered include: sound verbs, geminated verbs, assimilated verbs and verbs whose segments contain high vocoids. Both derived and non-derived trisegmental and quadrisegmental verbs are considered. Section 3 deals with previous account of the PP. Therein we expose two different approaches and show the limitations of each. In section 4, we present an alternative analysis couched within the OT framework. Here two hypotheses will be advanced to explain the behavior of the PP: the first says that there are prosodic

constraints on the PP verb stem; the second, which is adopted in the present work, maintains that these constraints hold on the PP word rather than the verb stem. Throughout the chapter, it will be argued that the analysis that works best for CMA is one where the output of the PP corresponds to an iambic foot of the type LH or else LL.

2. A TAXONOMIC SURVEY OF THE PASSIVE PARTICIPLE

This subsection will give a survey of all PP forms obtained from both derived and nonderived transitive verb bases. The word base is taken here to be a root, a stem or a word.

2.1The PP of Trisegmental Verb Bases

2.1.1Non-Derived Verbs

Consider the sets in 1 below. The base in 1a represents the class of sound verbs; 1b represents the class of verbs whose medial segment is one of the high vocoids /i/ or /u/; 1c represents the class of assimilated verbs; 1d is that of final geminated verbs, and finally 1e represents the class of verbs whose second and/or third segment is a glide or a high vocoid.

-1-

 

Base

PP

PP Gloss

a.

ktb

məktub

written

 

lʕb

məlʕub

played

 

ʃrb

məʃrubdrunk

 

b.

ʃuf

məʃyufseen

 

 

biʕ

məbyuʕ

sold

 

dir

mədyur

done

c.

wqf

məwquf

stopped

 

wzn

məwzun

weighed

 

wld

məwlud

born

d.

sdd 1

məsdud

locked

 

ʕDD

məʕDuD

bitten

 

hzz

məhzuz

taken

1 The final consonant in these items is doubled to show they behave like the rest of trisegmental verbs and also to encode the notion of the Two-Root Theory of geminates proposed in Selkirk (1990, 1991). See chapter two and section three in this chapter for details about the representation of geminates.

204

e.

ʃri

məʃri

bought

 

kri

məkri

rented

 

ʃwi

məʃwi

roasted

A comment about roots containing a high vocoid is in order. In chapter four, we assume, following Rosenthall (1994), that verbs with medial high vocoids such as /fiq/ and /dub/ are represented underlyingly with the vowels /i/ and /u/ and that the non-moraic realization of these vowels as glides in words such as [fəyyəq] and [dəwwəb] is the result of constraint interaction. This assumption works for verbs with medial vocoids as has already been shown in chapter four and also with verbs with final high vocoids such as /ʃri/, and /kri/ (Cf. [ʃərray] “buyer”and [kərray] “tenant”, where the input high vowel is realized as the glide [y]). Other examples that exhibit a vowel/glide alternation are given below:

-2-

 

 

 

 

 

Verb

Causative

Agent Noun

Gloss

a.

dub

dəwwəb

dissolve

 

ʃuf

ʃəwwəf

ʃəwwaf

see

 

bul

bəwwəl

bəwwal

urinate

 

gul

gəwwəl

gəwwal

say

 

nuD

nəwwəD

get up

 

fiq

fəyyəq

fəyyaq

wake up

 

Tiħ

Təyyəħ

fall down

 

biʕ

bəyyəʕ

bəyyaʕ

sell

b.

ʃri

ʃərray

buy

 

kri

kərray

rent

 

bki

bəkki

bəkkay

cry

 

ʒri

ʒərri

ʒərray

run

For all these cases where there is a vowel/glide alternation, we will continue to assume that high vocoids are underlyingly represented with /i/ and /u/.

As to the cases where the distribution of high vowels and glides cannot be attributed to an alternation between the two, Rosenthall (1994) posits glides in the underlying representation. This could be the case of trisegmental verbs whose initial segment is a high vocoid and verbs whose second and third segments are vocoids as shown in the examples below:

205

-3-

 

 

 

 

 

Vb Root

Vb Stem

 

Gloss

a.

wzn

wzən

*ʔuzən

weigh

 

wld

wləd

*?uləd

give birth to

 

wqf

wqəf

*?uqəf

stand

 

wSl

wSəl

*?uSəl

arrive

 

wħl

wħəl

*?uħəl

get stuck

 

ybs

ybəs

*?ibəs

get dry

b.

lwi

lwi

*luy

twist

 

kwi

kwi

*kuy

burn

 

dwi

dwi

*duy

speak

 

Twi

Twi

*Tuy

fold

 

nwi

nwi

*nuy

intend

The asterisked forms show that the initial glide in 3a and the medial glide in 3b never alternates with the corresponding high vowel, at least in CMA2. In 3a the initial glide can never surface as a vowel (the glottal stop is prothetic and serves as an onset). Similarly, the medial glide in 3b is always realized as non-moraic. For verbs like these ones, we assume that the glide is part of the underlying form.

The derived verb bases of the forms corresponding to the ones in 1a-e form their PP in the same way as the non-derived counterparts. Thus derived trisegmental verb bases such as [təktəb], [tʃədd] and [təwzən] have the forms [məktub], [məʃdud] and [məwzun] as their

respective PP.

The patterns of the PP forms of non-derived verbs are listed in 4 below. The period marks syllable boundary; G stands for glide; the subscript means that the two segments are identical and finally the letters correspond to those of the sets given in 1 above:

-4-

a.CəC.CuC

b.CəC.GuC

2 Benkaddour (1982) assumes that in Rabati MA, high vocoids are allowed to surface as vowels in initial position. Thus, for the author, forms such as [ʔuSəl] and [ʔibəs] are attested in this variety.

206

c.CəG.CuC

d.CəC.CiuCi

e.CəC.Ci / CəC.Gi

2.1.2 Derived Verb Bases

The forms in 5 below are derived either by medial gemination of the second segment of the root or by the prefixation of the medio-passive morpheme [t-] to the geminated form:

-5-

Vb Base

a.kəttəb tkəttəb ləʕʕəb tləʕʕəb

b.ʃəwwəf tʃəwwəf dəwwəz ddəwwəz

c.wəqqəf twəqqəf wəlləf twəlləf

d.rəqqəq trəqqəq nəZZəZ tnəZZəZ

e.ʕərri tʕərra 3 γəTTi TγəTTa

PP

PP Gloss

mkəttəb

written

mkəttəb

 

mləʕʕəb

played

mləʕʕəb

 

mʃəwwəf

seen

mʃəwwəf

 

mdəwwəz

examined

mdəwwəz

 

mwəqqəf

stopped

mwəqqəf

 

mwəlləf

be accustomed to

mwəlləf

 

mrəqqəq

made thin

mrəqqəq

 

mnəZZəZ

toasted

mnəZZəZ

 

mʕərri

uncovered

mʕərri

 

mγəTTi

covered

mγəTTi

 

3 Verbs of this type show an alternation between the vowel [i] and the vowel [a]. Thus they have two allomorphs; one with [i] as in the passive [mʕərri], the other with [a] as in [tʕərra]. (For i/a and u/a allomorphy in MA, the reader may refer to works such as Al Ghadi (1990), Bennis (1992), Boudlal (1993) and Meliani (1995).

207

Notice that the medio-passive morpheme [t-] takes on the voicing feature of the root initial consonant if it is coronal as is the case with the verb [ddəwwəz]. Note also that whether or not the mediopassive morpheme is prefixed to the base, the PP obtained is always the same. The patterns of the PP forms in 5 are listed below:

-6-

a.CCəCi.CiəC

b.CCəGi.GiəC

c.CGəCi.CiəC

d.CCəCi.CiəCi

e.CCəCi.Cii

There is a second category of the PP obtained from derived verb bases. These verb bases are themselves derived by the infixation of the consonant [-t-] (and possibly the vowel [-a-] that follows it) after the first segment as shown below:

-7-

 

 

 

 

Vb Base

PP

PP Gloss

a.

htəmm

məhtəmm

be interested in

 

ħtəʒʒ

məħtəʒʒ

protesting

b.

ħtaʒ

məħtaʒ

be in need of

 

rtaħ

mərtaħ

relaxed

c.

xtarəʕ

məxtarəʕ

invented

 

ħtarəm

məħtarəm

respected

Most of the forms in 7 are classicized forms, reported to be used in MMA (Youssi, 1992). The patterns of the items in 7 are given in 8:

-8-

a.CəC.CəCiCi

b.CəC.CaC

c.CəC.Ca. CəC

208

Соседние файлы в предмете [НЕСОРТИРОВАННОЕ]