
- •Preface
- •Approach and Pedagogy
- •Chapter 1
- •Introducing Psychology
- •1.1 Psychology as a Science
- •The Problem of Intuition
- •Research Focus: Unconscious Preferences for the Letters of Our Own Name
- •Why Psychologists Rely on Empirical Methods
- •Levels of Explanation in Psychology
- •The Challenges of Studying Psychology
- •1.2 The Evolution of Psychology: History, Approaches, and Questions
- •Early Psychologists
- •Structuralism: Introspection and the Awareness of Subjective Experience
- •Functionalism and Evolutionary Psychology
- •Psychodynamic Psychology
- •Behaviorism and the Question of Free Will
- •Research Focus: Do We Have Free Will?
- •The Cognitive Approach and Cognitive Neuroscience
- •The War of the Ghosts
- •Social-Cultural Psychology
- •The Many Disciplines of Psychology
- •Psychology in Everyday Life: How to Effectively Learn and Remember
- •1.3 Chapter Summary
- •Chapter 2
- •Psychological Science
- •Psychological Journals
- •2.1 Psychologists Use the Scientific Method to Guide Their Research
- •The Scientific Method
- •Laws and Theories as Organizing Principles
- •The Research Hypothesis
- •Conducting Ethical Research
- •Characteristics of an Ethical Research Project Using Human Participants
- •Ensuring That Research Is Ethical
- •Research With Animals
- •APA Guidelines on Humane Care and Use of Animals in Research
- •Descriptive Research: Assessing the Current State of Affairs
- •Correlational Research: Seeking Relationships Among Variables
- •Experimental Research: Understanding the Causes of Behavior
- •Research Focus: Video Games and Aggression
- •2.3 You Can Be an Informed Consumer of Psychological Research
- •Threats to the Validity of Research
- •Psychology in Everyday Life: Critically Evaluating the Validity of Websites
- •2.4 Chapter Summary
- •Chapter 3
- •Brains, Bodies, and Behavior
- •Did a Neurological Disorder Cause a Musician to Compose Boléro and an Artist to Paint It 66 Years Later?
- •3.1 The Neuron Is the Building Block of the Nervous System
- •Neurons Communicate Using Electricity and Chemicals
- •Video Clip: The Electrochemical Action of the Neuron
- •Neurotransmitters: The Body’s Chemical Messengers
- •3.2 Our Brains Control Our Thoughts, Feelings, and Behavior
- •The Old Brain: Wired for Survival
- •The Cerebral Cortex Creates Consciousness and Thinking
- •Functions of the Cortex
- •The Brain Is Flexible: Neuroplasticity
- •Research Focus: Identifying the Unique Functions of the Left and Right Hemispheres Using Split-Brain Patients
- •Psychology in Everyday Life: Why Are Some People Left-Handed?
- •3.3 Psychologists Study the Brain Using Many Different Methods
- •Lesions Provide a Picture of What Is Missing
- •Recording Electrical Activity in the Brain
- •Peeking Inside the Brain: Neuroimaging
- •Research Focus: Cyberostracism
- •3.4 Putting It All Together: The Nervous System and the Endocrine System
- •Electrical Control of Behavior: The Nervous System
- •The Body’s Chemicals Help Control Behavior: The Endocrine System
- •3.5 Chapter Summary
- •Chapter 4
- •Sensing and Perceiving
- •Misperception by Those Trained to Accurately Perceive a Threat
- •4.1 We Experience Our World Through Sensation
- •Sensory Thresholds: What Can We Experience?
- •Link
- •Measuring Sensation
- •Research Focus: Influence without Awareness
- •4.2 Seeing
- •The Sensing Eye and the Perceiving Visual Cortex
- •Perceiving Color
- •Perceiving Form
- •Perceiving Depth
- •Perceiving Motion
- •Beta Effect and Phi Phenomenon
- •4.3 Hearing
- •Hearing Loss
- •4.4 Tasting, Smelling, and Touching
- •Tasting
- •Smelling
- •Touching
- •Experiencing Pain
- •4.5 Accuracy and Inaccuracy in Perception
- •How the Perceptual System Interprets the Environment
- •Video Clip: The McGurk Effect
- •Video Clip: Selective Attention
- •Illusions
- •The Important Role of Expectations in Perception
- •Psychology in Everyday Life: How Understanding Sensation and Perception Can Save Lives
- •4.6 Chapter Summary
- •Chapter 5
- •States of Consciousness
- •An Unconscious Killing
- •5.1 Sleeping and Dreaming Revitalize Us for Action
- •Research Focus: Circadian Rhythms Influence the Use of Stereotypes in Social Judgments
- •Sleep Stages: Moving Through the Night
- •Sleep Disorders: Problems in Sleeping
- •The Heavy Costs of Not Sleeping
- •Dreams and Dreaming
- •5.2 Altering Consciousness With Psychoactive Drugs
- •Speeding Up the Brain With Stimulants: Caffeine, Nicotine, Cocaine, and Amphetamines
- •Slowing Down the Brain With Depressants: Alcohol, Barbiturates and Benzodiazepines, and Toxic Inhalants
- •Opioids: Opium, Morphine, Heroin, and Codeine
- •Hallucinogens: Cannabis, Mescaline, and LSD
- •Why We Use Psychoactive Drugs
- •Research Focus: Risk Tolerance Predicts Cigarette Use
- •5.3 Altering Consciousness Without Drugs
- •Changing Behavior Through Suggestion: The Power of Hypnosis
- •Reducing Sensation to Alter Consciousness: Sensory Deprivation
- •Meditation
- •Video Clip: Try Meditation
- •Psychology in Everyday Life: The Need to Escape Everyday Consciousness
- •5.4 Chapter Summary
- •Chapter 6
- •Growing and Developing
- •The Repository for Germinal Choice
- •6.1 Conception and Prenatal Development
- •The Zygote
- •The Embryo
- •The Fetus
- •How the Environment Can Affect the Vulnerable Fetus
- •6.2 Infancy and Childhood: Exploring and Learning
- •The Newborn Arrives With Many Behaviors Intact
- •Research Focus: Using the Habituation Technique to Study What Infants Know
- •Cognitive Development During Childhood
- •Video Clip: Object Permanence
- •Social Development During Childhood
- •Knowing the Self: The Development of the Self-Concept
- •Video Clip: The Harlows’ Monkeys
- •Video Clip: The Strange Situation
- •Research Focus: Using a Longitudinal Research Design to Assess the Stability of Attachment
- •6.3 Adolescence: Developing Independence and Identity
- •Physical Changes in Adolescence
- •Cognitive Development in Adolescence
- •Social Development in Adolescence
- •Developing Moral Reasoning: Kohlberg’s Theory
- •Video Clip: People Being Interviewed About Kohlberg’s Stages
- •6.4 Early and Middle Adulthood: Building Effective Lives
- •Psychology in Everyday Life: What Makes a Good Parent?
- •Physical and Cognitive Changes in Early and Middle Adulthood
- •Menopause
- •Social Changes in Early and Middle Adulthood
- •6.5 Late Adulthood: Aging, Retiring, and Bereavement
- •Cognitive Changes During Aging
- •Dementia and Alzheimer’s Disease
- •Social Changes During Aging: Retiring Effectively
- •Death, Dying, and Bereavement
- •6.6 Chapter Summary
- •Chapter 7
- •Learning
- •My Story of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
- •7.1 Learning by Association: Classical Conditioning
- •Pavlov Demonstrates Conditioning in Dogs
- •The Persistence and Extinction of Conditioning
- •The Role of Nature in Classical Conditioning
- •How Reinforcement and Punishment Influence Behavior: The Research of Thorndike and Skinner
- •Video Clip: Thorndike’s Puzzle Box
- •Creating Complex Behaviors Through Operant Conditioning
- •7.3 Learning by Insight and Observation
- •Observational Learning: Learning by Watching
- •Video Clip: Bandura Discussing Clips From His Modeling Studies
- •Research Focus: The Effects of Violent Video Games on Aggression
- •7.4 Using the Principles of Learning to Understand Everyday Behavior
- •Using Classical Conditioning in Advertising
- •Video Clip: Television Ads
- •Psychology in Everyday Life: Operant Conditioning in the Classroom
- •Reinforcement in Social Dilemmas
- •7.5 Chapter Summary
- •Chapter 8
- •Remembering and Judging
- •She Was Certain, but She Was Wrong
- •Differences between Brains and Computers
- •Video Clip: Kim Peek
- •8.1 Memories as Types and Stages
- •Explicit Memory
- •Implicit Memory
- •Research Focus: Priming Outside Awareness Influences Behavior
- •Stages of Memory: Sensory, Short-Term, and Long-Term Memory
- •Sensory Memory
- •Short-Term Memory
- •8.2 How We Remember: Cues to Improving Memory
- •Encoding and Storage: How Our Perceptions Become Memories
- •Research Focus: Elaboration and Memory
- •Using the Contributions of Hermann Ebbinghaus to Improve Your Memory
- •Retrieval
- •Retrieval Demonstration
- •States and Capital Cities
- •The Structure of LTM: Categories, Prototypes, and Schemas
- •The Biology of Memory
- •8.3 Accuracy and Inaccuracy in Memory and Cognition
- •Source Monitoring: Did It Really Happen?
- •Schematic Processing: Distortions Based on Expectations
- •Misinformation Effects: How Information That Comes Later Can Distort Memory
- •Overconfidence
- •Heuristic Processing: Availability and Representativeness
- •Salience and Cognitive Accessibility
- •Counterfactual Thinking
- •Psychology in Everyday Life: Cognitive Biases in the Real World
- •8.4 Chapter Summary
- •Chapter 9
- •Intelligence and Language
- •How We Talk (or Do Not Talk) about Intelligence
- •9.1 Defining and Measuring Intelligence
- •General (g) Versus Specific (s) Intelligences
- •Measuring Intelligence: Standardization and the Intelligence Quotient
- •The Biology of Intelligence
- •Is Intelligence Nature or Nurture?
- •Psychology in Everyday Life: Emotional Intelligence
- •9.2 The Social, Cultural, and Political Aspects of Intelligence
- •Extremes of Intelligence: Retardation and Giftedness
- •Extremely Low Intelligence
- •Extremely High Intelligence
- •Sex Differences in Intelligence
- •Racial Differences in Intelligence
- •Research Focus: Stereotype Threat
- •9.3 Communicating With Others: The Development and Use of Language
- •The Components of Language
- •Examples in Which Syntax Is Correct but the Interpretation Can Be Ambiguous
- •The Biology and Development of Language
- •Research Focus: When Can We Best Learn Language? Testing the Critical Period Hypothesis
- •Learning Language
- •How Children Learn Language: Theories of Language Acquisition
- •Bilingualism and Cognitive Development
- •Can Animals Learn Language?
- •Video Clip: Language Recognition in Bonobos
- •Language and Perception
- •9.4 Chapter Summary
- •Chapter 10
- •Emotions and Motivations
- •Captain Sullenberger Conquers His Emotions
- •10.1 The Experience of Emotion
- •Video Clip: The Basic Emotions
- •The Cannon-Bard and James-Lange Theories of Emotion
- •Research Focus: Misattributing Arousal
- •Communicating Emotion
- •10.2 Stress: The Unseen Killer
- •The Negative Effects of Stress
- •Stressors in Our Everyday Lives
- •Responses to Stress
- •Managing Stress
- •Emotion Regulation
- •Research Focus: Emotion Regulation Takes Effort
- •10.3 Positive Emotions: The Power of Happiness
- •Finding Happiness Through Our Connections With Others
- •What Makes Us Happy?
- •10.4 Two Fundamental Human Motivations: Eating and Mating
- •Eating: Healthy Choices Make Healthy Lives
- •Obesity
- •Sex: The Most Important Human Behavior
- •The Experience of Sex
- •The Many Varieties of Sexual Behavior
- •Psychology in Everyday Life: Regulating Emotions to Improve Our Health
- •10.5 Chapter Summary
- •Chapter 11
- •Personality
- •Identical Twins Reunited after 35 Years
- •11.1 Personality and Behavior: Approaches and Measurement
- •Personality as Traits
- •Example of a Trait Measure
- •Situational Influences on Personality
- •The MMPI and Projective Tests
- •Psychology in Everyday Life: Leaders and Leadership
- •11.2 The Origins of Personality
- •Psychodynamic Theories of Personality: The Role of the Unconscious
- •Id, Ego, and Superego
- •Research Focus: How the Fear of Death Causes Aggressive Behavior
- •Strengths and Limitations of Freudian and Neo-Freudian Approaches
- •Focusing on the Self: Humanism and Self-Actualization
- •Research Focus: Self-Discrepancies, Anxiety, and Depression
- •Studying Personality Using Behavioral Genetics
- •Studying Personality Using Molecular Genetics
- •Reviewing the Literature: Is Our Genetics Our Destiny?
- •11.4 Chapter Summary
- •Chapter 12
- •Defining Psychological Disorders
- •When Minor Body Imperfections Lead to Suicide
- •12.1 Psychological Disorder: What Makes a Behavior “Abnormal”?
- •Defining Disorder
- •Psychology in Everyday Life: Combating the Stigma of Abnormal Behavior
- •Diagnosing Disorder: The DSM
- •Diagnosis or Overdiagnosis? ADHD, Autistic Disorder, and Asperger’s Disorder
- •Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)
- •Autistic Disorder and Asperger’s Disorder
- •12.2 Anxiety and Dissociative Disorders: Fearing the World Around Us
- •Generalized Anxiety Disorder
- •Panic Disorder
- •Phobias
- •Obsessive-Compulsive Disorders
- •Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD)
- •Dissociative Disorders: Losing the Self to Avoid Anxiety
- •Dissociative Amnesia and Fugue
- •Dissociative Identity Disorder
- •Explaining Anxiety and Dissociation Disorders
- •12.3 Mood Disorders: Emotions as Illness
- •Behaviors Associated with Depression
- •Dysthymia and Major Depressive Disorder
- •Bipolar Disorder
- •Explaining Mood Disorders
- •Research Focus: Using Molecular Genetics to Unravel the Causes of Depression
- •12.4 Schizophrenia: The Edge of Reality and Consciousness
- •Symptoms of Schizophrenia
- •Explaining Schizophrenia
- •12.5 Personality Disorders
- •Borderline Personality Disorder
- •Research Focus: Affective and Cognitive Deficits in BPD
- •Antisocial Personality Disorder (APD)
- •12.6 Somatoform, Factitious, and Sexual Disorders
- •Somatoform and Factitious Disorders
- •Sexual Disorders
- •Disorders of Sexual Function
- •Paraphilias
- •12.7 Chapter Summary
- •Chapter 13
- •Treating Psychological Disorders
- •Therapy on Four Legs
- •13.1 Reducing Disorder by Confronting It: Psychotherapy
- •DSM-IV-TR Criteria for Diagnosing Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)
- •Psychology in Everyday Life: Seeking Treatment for Psychological Difficulties
- •Psychodynamic Therapy
- •Important Characteristics and Experiences in Psychoanalysis
- •Humanistic Therapies
- •Behavioral Aspects of CBT
- •Cognitive Aspects of CBT
- •Combination (Eclectic) Approaches to Therapy
- •13.2 Reducing Disorder Biologically: Drug and Brain Therapy
- •Drug Therapies
- •Using Stimulants to Treat ADHD
- •Antidepressant Medications
- •Antianxiety Medications
- •Antipsychotic Medications
- •Direct Brain Intervention Therapies
- •13.3 Reducing Disorder by Changing the Social Situation
- •Group, Couples, and Family Therapy
- •Self-Help Groups
- •Community Mental Health: Service and Prevention
- •Some Risk Factors for Psychological Disorders
- •Research Focus: The Implicit Association Test as a Behavioral Marker for Suicide
- •13.4 Evaluating Treatment and Prevention: What Works?
- •Effectiveness of Psychological Therapy
- •Research Focus: Meta-Analyzing Clinical Outcomes
- •Effectiveness of Biomedical Therapies
- •Effectiveness of Social-Community Approaches
- •13.5 Chapter Summary
- •Chapter 14
- •Psychology in Our Social Lives
- •Binge Drinking and the Death of a Homecoming Queen
- •14.1 Social Cognition: Making Sense of Ourselvesand Others
- •Perceiving Others
- •Forming Judgments on the Basis of Appearance: Stereotyping, Prejudice, and Discrimination
- •Implicit Association Test
- •Research Focus: Forming Judgments of People in Seconds
- •Close Relationships
- •Causal Attribution: Forming Judgments by Observing Behavior
- •Attitudes and Behavior
- •14.2 Interacting With Others: Helping, Hurting, and Conforming
- •Helping Others: Altruism Helps Create Harmonious Relationships
- •Why Are We Altruistic?
- •How the Presence of Others Can Reduce Helping
- •Video Clip: The Case of Kitty Genovese
- •Human Aggression: An Adaptive yet Potentially Damaging Behavior
- •The Ability to Aggress Is Part of Human Nature
- •Negative Experiences Increase Aggression
- •Viewing Violent Media Increases Aggression
- •Video Clip
- •Research Focus: The Culture of Honor
- •Conformity and Obedience: How Social Influence Creates Social Norms
- •Video Clip
- •Do We Always Conform?
- •14.3 Working With Others: The Costs and Benefits of Social Groups
- •Working in Front of Others: Social Facilitation and Social Inhibition
- •Working Together in Groups
- •Psychology in Everyday Life: Do Juries Make Good Decisions?
- •Using Groups Effectively
- •14.4 Chapter Summary

Misinformation Effects: How Information That Comes Later Can Distort Memory
A particular problem for eyewitnesses such as Jennifer Thompson is that our memories are often influenced by the things that occur to us after we have learned the information (Erdmann, Volbert, & Böhm, 2004; Loftus, 1979; Zaragoza, Belli, & Payment, 2007). [10] This new information can distort our original memories such that the we are no longer sure what is the real information and what was provided later. The misinformation effect refers to errors in memory that occur when new information influences existing memories.
In an experiment by Loftus and Palmer (1974), [11] participants viewed a film of a traffic accident and then, according to random assignment to experimental conditions, answered one of three questions:
“About how fast were the cars going when they hit each other?”
“About how fast were the cars going when they smashed each other?”
“About how fast were the cars going when they contacted each other?”
As you can see in Figure 8.20 "Misinformation Effect", although all the participants saw the same accident, their estimates of the cars’ speed varied by condition. Participants who had been asked about the cars “smashing” each other estimated the highest average speed, and those who had been asked the “contacted” question estimated the lowest average speed.
Saylor URL: http://www.saylor.org/books |
Saylor.org |
|
399 |

Figure 8.20 Misinformation Effect
Participants viewed a film of a traffic accident and then answered a question about the accident. According to random assignment, the verb in the question was filled by either “hit,” “smashed,” or “contacted” each other. The wording of the question influenced the participants’ memory of the accident.
Source: Adapted from Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 13(5), 585–589.
In addition to distorting our memories for events that have actually occurred, misinformation may lead us to falsely remember information that never occurred. Loftus and her colleagues asked parents to provide them with descriptions of events that did (e.g., moving to a new house) and did not (e.g., being lost in a shopping mall) happen to their children. Then (without telling the children which events were real or made-up) the researchers asked the children to imagine both types of events. The children were instructed to “think real hard” about whether the events had occurred (Ceci, Huffman, Smith, & Loftus, 1994). [12] More than half of the children generated stories regarding at least one of the made-up events, and they remained insistent that the events did in fact occur even when told by the researcher that they could not possibly have occurred (Loftus & Pickrell, 1995). [13] Even college students are susceptible to manipulations that make events that did not actually occur seem as if they did (Mazzoni, Loftus, & Kirsch, 2001). [14]
The ease with which memories can be created or implanted is particularly problematic when the events to be recalled have important consequences. Therapists often argue that patients may repress memories of traumatic events they experienced as children, such as childhood sexual
Saylor URL: http://www.saylor.org/books |
Saylor.org |
|
400 |

abuse, and then recover the events years later as the therapist leads them to recall the information—for instance, by using dream interpretation and hypnosis (Brown, Scheflin, & Hammond, 1998). [15]
But other researchers argue that painful memories such as sexual abuse are usually very well remembered, that few memories are actually repressed, and that even if they are it is virtually impossible for patients to accurately retrieve them years later (McNally, Bryant, & Ehlers, 2003; Pope, Poliakoff, Parker, Boynes, & Hudson, 2007). [16] These researchers have argued that the procedures used by the therapists to “retrieve” the memories are more likely to actually implant false memories, leading the patients to erroneously recall events that did not actually occur. Because hundreds of people have been accused, and even imprisoned, on the basis of claims about “recovered memory” of child sexual abuse, the accuracy of these memories has important societal implications. Many psychologists now believe that most of these claims of recovered memories are due to implanted, rather than real, memories (Loftus & Ketcham, 1994). [17]
Overconfidence
One of the most remarkable aspects of Jennifer Thompson’s mistaken identity of Ronald Cotton was her certainty. But research reveals a pervasive cognitive bias toward overconfidence, which is the tendency for people to be too certain about their ability to accurately remember events and to make judgments. David Dunning and his colleagues (Dunning, Griffin, Milojkovic, & Ross, 1990) [18] asked college students to predict how another student would react in various situations. Some participants made predictions about a fellow student whom they had just met and interviewed, and others made predictions about their roommates whom they knew very well. In both cases, participants reported their confidence in each prediction, and accuracy was determined by the responses of the people themselves. The results were clear: Regardless of whether they judged a stranger or a roommate, the participants consistently overestimated the accuracy of their own predictions.
Eyewitnesses to crimes are also frequently overconfident in their memories, and there is only a small correlation between how accurate and how confident an eyewitness is. The witness who claims to be absolutely certain about his or her identification (e.g., Jennifer Thompson) is not
Saylor URL: http://www.saylor.org/books |
Saylor.org |
|
401 |

much more likely to be accurate than one who appears much less sure, making it almost impossible to determine whether a particular witness is accurate or not (Wells & Olson, 2003). [19]
I am sure that you have a clear memory of when you first heard about the 9/11 attacks in 2001, and perhaps also when you heard that Princess Diana was killed in 1997 or when the verdict of the O. J. Simpson trial was announced in 1995. This type of memory, which we experience along with a great deal of emotion, is known as a flashbulb memory—a vivid and emotional memory of an unusual event that people believe they remember very well. (Brown & Kulik, 1977). [20]
People are very certain of their memories of these important events, and frequently overconfident. Talarico and Rubin (2003) [21] tested the accuracy of flashbulb memories by asking students to write down their memory of how they had heard the news about either the September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks or about an everyday event that had occurred to them during the same time frame. These recordings were made on September 12, 2001. Then the participants were asked again, either 1, 6, or 32 weeks later, to recall their memories. The participants became less accurate in their recollections of both the emotional event and the everyday events over time. But the participants’ confidence in the accuracy of their memory of learning about the attacks did not decline over time. After 32 weeks the participants were overconfident; they were much more certain about the accuracy of their flashbulb memories than they should have been. Schmolck, Buffalo, and Squire (2000) [22] found similar distortions in memories of news about the verdict in the O. J. Simpson trial.
Heuristic Processing: Availability and Representativeness
Another way that our information processing may be biased occurs when we use heuristics, which are information-processing strategies that are useful in many cases but may lead to errors when misapplied. Let’s consider two of the most frequently applied (and misapplied) heuristics: the representativeness heuristic and the availability heuristic.
In many cases we base our judgments on information that seems to represent, or match, what we expect will happen, while ignoring other potentially more relevant statistical information. When we do so, we are using the representativeness heuristic. Consider, for instance, the puzzle
Saylor URL: http://www.saylor.org/books |
Saylor.org |
|
402 |

presented in Table 8.4 "The Representativeness Heuristic". Let’s say that you went to a hospital, and you checked the records of the babies that were born today. Which pattern of births do you think you are most likely to find?
Table 8.4 The Representativeness Heuristic
List A |
|
|
List B |
|
|
|
|
|
|
6:31 a.m. |
Girl |
6:31 a.m. |
|
Boy |
|
||||
|
|
|
|
|
8:15 a.m. |
Girl |
8:15 a.m. |
|
Girl |
|
|
|
|
|
9:42 a.m. |
Girl |
9:42 a.m. |
|
Boy |
|
|
|
|
|
1:13 p.m. |
Girl |
1:13 p.m. |
|
Girl |
|
|
|
|
|
3:39 p.m. |
Boy |
3:39 p.m. |
|
Girl |
|
|
|
|
|
5:12 p.m. |
Boy |
5:12 p.m. |
|
Boy |
|
|
|
|
|
7:42 p.m. |
Boy |
7:42 p.m. |
|
Girl |
|
|
|
|
|
11:44 p.m. |
Boy |
11:44 p.m. |
|
Boy |
|
|
|
|
|
Using the representativeness heuristic may lead us to incorrectly believe that some patterns of observed events are more likely to have occurred than others. In this case, list B seems more random, and thus is judged as more likely to have occurred, but statistically both lists are equally likely.
Most people think that list B is more likely, probably because list B looks more random, and thus matches (is “representative of”) our ideas about randomness. But statisticians know that any pattern of four girls and four boys is mathematically equally likely. The problem is that we have a schema of what randomness should be like, which doesn’t always match what is mathematically the case. Similarly, people who see a flipped coin come up “heads” five times in a row will frequently predict, and perhaps even wager money, that “tails” will be next. This behavior is known as the gambler’s fallacy. But mathematically, the gambler’s fallacy is an error: The likelihood of any single coin flip being “tails” is always 50%, regardless of how many times it has come up “heads” in the past.
Our judgments can also be influenced by how easy it is to retrieve a memory. The tendency to make judgments of the frequency or likelihood that an event occurs on the basis of the ease with which it can be retrieved from memory is known as the availability heuristic (MacLeod &
Saylor URL: http://www.saylor.org/books |
Saylor.org |
|
403 |