- •Irony 133
- •Interjections and Exclamatory Words 140
- •Part I introduction
- •I. General notes on style and Stylistics
- •2. Expressive means (em) and stylistic devices (sd)
- •3. General notes on functional styles of language
- •4. Varieties of language
- •5. A brief outline of the development of the english literary (standard) language
- •6. Meaning from a stylistic point of view
- •Part II stylistic classification of the english vocabulary
- •I. General considerations
- •In accordance with the already-mentioned division of language into literary and colloquial, we may represent the whole of the word-stock
- •2. Neutral, common literary and common colloquial vocabulary
- •3. Special literary vocabulary a) Terms
- •B) Poetic and Highly Literary Words
- •C) Archaic, Obsolescent and Obsolete Words
- •D) Barbarisms and Foreignisms
- •E) Literary Coinages (Including Nonce-Words)
- •4. Special colloquial vocabulary a) Slang
- •B) Jargonisms
- •C) Professionalisms
- •D) Dialectal words
- •E) Vulgar words or vulgarisms
- •F) Colloquial coinages (words and meanings)
- •Part ш phonetic expressive means and stylistic devices general notes
- •Onomatopoeia
- •Alliteration
- •Part IV lexical expressive means and stylistic devices a. Intentional mixing of the stylistic aspect of words
- •B. Interaction of different types of lexical meaning
- •1. Interaction of primary dictionary and contextually imposed meanings
- •Metaphor
- •Metonymy
- •2. Interaction of primary and derivative logical meanings Stylistic Devices Based on Polysemantic Effect, Zeugma and Pun
- •3. Interaction of logical and emotive meanings
- •Interjections and Exclamatory Words
- •The Epithet
- •Oxymoron
- •4. Interaction of logical and nominal meanings Antonomasia
- •C. Intensification of a certain feature of a thing or phenomenon
- •Periphrasis
- •Euphemism
- •Hyperbole
- •D. Peculiar use of set expressions
- •The Cliche
- •Proverbs and Sayings
- •Epigrams
- •Allusions
- •Decomposition of Set Phrases
- •Part V syntactical expressive means and stylistic devices a. General considerations
- •B. Problems concerning the composition of spans of utterance larger than the sentence
- •Supra-Phrasal Units
- •The Paragraph
- •C. Compositional patterns of syntactical arrangement
- •Stylistic Inversion
- •Detached Construction
- •Parallel Construction
- •Chiasmus (Reversed Parallel Construction)
- •Repetition
- •Enumeration
- •Suspense
- •Climax (Gradation)
- •Antithesis
- •D. Particular ways of combining parts of the utterance (linkage)
- •Asyndeton
- •Polysyndeton
- •E. Particular use of colloquial constructions
- •Ellipsis
- •Break-in-the-Narrative (Appsiopesis)
- •Question-in-the-Narrative
- •Represented Speech
- •A) Uttered Represented Speech
- •B) Unuttered or Inner Represented Speech
- •F. Stylistic use of structural meaning
- •Rhetorical Questions
- •Litotes
- •Part VI functional styles of the english language
- •Introductory remarks
- •A. The belles-lettres style
- •1. Language of poetry
- •A) Compositional Patterns of Rhythmical Arrangement Metre and Line
- •The Stanza
- •Free Verse and Accented Verse
- •B) Lexical and Syntactical Features of Verse
- •2. Emotive prose
- •3. Language of the drama
- •B. Publicists style
- •1. Oratory and speeches
- •2. The essay
- •3. Journalistic articles
- •C. Newspaper style
- •1. Brief news items
- •2. Advertisements and announcements
- •3. The headline
- •4. The editorial
- •D. Scientific prose style
- •E. The style of official documents
- •Final remarks
B. Problems concerning the composition of spans of utterance larger than the sentence
In recent years a new theory concerning the inner relations between context and form within the sentence has appeared. This theory, elaborated by S. Harris, N. Chomsky, M. Postal and others, is called Generative Grammar. It maintains that grammar must not only describe the laws which regulate the functioning of linguistic units but must also be capable of generating new sentences.
"A grammar of this kind," writes John Lyons, "is 'predictive' in that it establishes as grammatical, not only 'actual' sentences, but also 'potential' sentences." l
The reference to Lyons's statement has direct bearing on the problems of stylistic syntax. The fact is, as will be seen later, that any one of the syntactical SDs is capable of generating an unlimited number of sentences within the given pattern. However, according to orthodox generative grammar, some of them are regarded as 'ill-formed' and even 'ungram-matical' inasmuch as they fail to meet the requirements of the basic (kernel) structures.
The theory further maintains that there are two kinds of structures — a deep structure and a surface structure. The latter are the actual sentences produced by the former, which is not presented in language units and therefore unobservable.
Mention of this theory is made here, firstly, because in modern sty-listics attempts are being made to build up a grammar which would generate deviant constructions and thus broaden the limits of the 'well-formed' sentences which are regarded as the only ones that are 'grammatical'. Another reason is that transformation, one of the basic methods employed in generative grammars, is very effectively used in stylistics when it is necessary to find the stylistic meaning of this or that sentence structure. A third reason is that generative grammars aim at reconstructing the processes connected with the formation of sentences. This has direct bearing on the interpretation of syntactical SDs and particularly on their linguistic nature.
This theory enables the interpreter to look at a sentence from the point of view of what is 'behind' the sentence.
As J. P. Thorne states, "Generative grammar is important to stylistics because in addition to these'surface structure' facts, it is concerned
with the so-called 'deep structure' aspects of language, that is, those facts about linguistic structure which cannot be directly related to what can be observed. Most stylistic judgements relate to deep structure."1
It follows then that the so-called generative grammar is not so strikingly new. This is also noted by the well-known linguists John Lyons and D. Bolinger, 2 who state positively that there is nothing new in the theory of generative grammar.
Another development in linguistics also having direct bearing on the problems which concern us when dealing with syntactical SDs, is 'text-linguistics', as it is called. This development, which as yet has not been formed as a separate theory, aims at investigating the objective criteria concerning ways and means of constructing texts of different kinds and genres. 3
For this purpose it is first of all necessary to find the elements into which any text may fall. In other words, there must be certain constituent units of which any text is composed.
Phonemes, the smallest language units, function within morphemes and are dependent on them, morphemes function within words, words — within sentences, and sentences function in larger structural frames which we shall call "supra-phrasal units". Consequently, neither words nor separate sentences can be regarded as the basic constituents of a text. They are the basic units of lower levels of language-as-a-system, as is shown above.