
- •Cross-cultural universal traits and the culturally specific in human behavior Cross-cultural and Cultural Psychology Knud s. Larsen
- •Introduction
- •Introduction
- •Cross-cultural psychology in a changing world
- •Behavior as culture specific or universal.
- •1.2 The etic and emic approaches.
- •1.3 Cross-cultural psychology and cultural/ indigenous psychology.
- •1.4 Culture versus ethnicity and race.
- •1.5 All groups with a significant history have culture
- •1.6 Toward an inclusive definition of culture.
- •1.6.1 Culture is the evolution of human society.
- •1.6.2 Animal and human culture.
- •1.6.3 The ecological and sociological context.
- •1.7 Resource rich or poor cultures.
- •1.8 Cultural values and dimensions.
- •1.8.1 Universal values.
- •1.8.2 Cultural value dimensions.
- •1.8.3 The social axioms of Leung and Bond.
- •1.9 Enculturation, culture, and psychological outcomes.
- •1.10 Understanding cross-cultural psychology in a changing world.
- •1.11 The major objectives of cross-cultural psychology.
- •1.12 The ethnocentrism of Psychology.
- •Summary
- •Research approaches and critical thinking in cross-cultural psychology
- •2.1 Cultural bias and criterion of equivalence.
- •2.1.1 The issue of language equivalence.
- •2.1.2 Psychometric equivalence.
- •2.1.3 Selecting equivalent samples in cross-cultural psychology.
- •2.2 Nonequivalence in cross-cultural research.
- •2.3 Levels of inference.
- •2.4 Studies of cultural level ecological averages.
- •2.5 What is measured in cross-cultural research?
- •2.6 Bias in psychological assessments.
- •2.7 Inferences from statistical tests on cross-cultural comparisons.
- •2.8 Experimental versus correlational studies.
- •2.9 Qualitative and quantitative research in cross-cultural psychology.
- •2.10 Quantitative comparative cross-cultural research.
- •2.10.1 Surveys.
- •2.10.2 Experiments.
- •2.11 The problems of validity.
- •2.12 A critical look at the findings from cross-cultural comparisons.
- •2.13 Skeptical thinking is the path to an improved cross-cultural psychology.
- •Summary
- •The origin of culture: cultural transformation and sociocultural evolution
- •3.1 The case for the biological foundations of human characteristics.
- •3.1.1 Evolution and the mechanisms of transmission.
- •3.1.2 Races as a biological and social construct.
- •3.1.3 The role of adaptation.
- •3.2 The research supporting the evolution of human emotion.
- •3.2.1 Universal temperament and personality traits are evidence of common evolved history.
- •3.2.2 Intelligence as a biological and racial construct.
- •3.2.3 Behavior genetics and disease.
- •3.2.4 Hardwired optimism: The driver for cultural development.
- •3.3 Sociobiology and evolutionary psychology.
- •3.3.1 Gender differences in mate selection.
- •3.3.2 Is ethnocentrism and racism a broader manifestation of inclusive fitness for reproductive success?
- •3.4 Culture matters!
- •3.5 Socio-cultural evolution: a little history.
- •3.5.1 The evolution of evolutionary theories.
- •3.5.2 Dual inheritance: Approaches to cultural transmission.
- •3.6 Theories of modernization and post-industrial society.
- •Summary
- •Human development: culture and biology
- •4.1 Socialization or enculturation?
- •4.2 Enculturation and choice.
- •4.3 Authoritative versus authoritarian childrearing approaches and cultural differences.
- •4.4 Creating the climate of home: Cultural and cross-cultural studies.
- •4.4.1 The sleeping arrangements of childhood.
- •4.4.2 Attachment in childhood.
- •4.4.3 Relationships with siblings.
- •4.4.4 The influence of the extended family and peers.
- •4.5 Culture and the educational system.
- •4.6 Socio-economic climate.
- •4.7 Social identity.
- •4.8 Comparative studies in child rearing behaviors.
- •4.9 Human development is incorporation of culture.
- •4.10 Stage theories of human development: Culturally unique or universal.
- •4.10.1 The evolution of cognition.
- •4.10.2 The evolution of moral development.
- •4.10.3 Evolution of psychosocial development.
- •4.11 Human development is the expression of biology: the presence of universal values.
- •4.12 The evolutionary basis for human behavior: Maximizing inclusive fitness.
- •4.13 Perspective in the transmission of culture.
- •Summary
- •The evolution of language and socio-culture
- •5.1 The evolution of socioculture and language.
- •5.2 Language development: the meaning of language terms and early speech.
- •5.3 Cultural language difference and linguistic relativity.
- •5.4 Cultural language and thought.
- •5.5 Universals in language.
- •5.6 Intercultural communication.
- •5.6.1 Obstacles and uncertainty reduction in intercultural communication.
- •5.6.2 The affect of bilingualism.
- •5.7 Nonverbal communication and culture.
- •5.8 Darwinian evolution and phylogenetic trees of language and socio-cultural evolution.
- •5.8.1 Selective group genetic advantages in cultural evolution.
- •5.8.2 The analogy of genetic and cultural evolution.
- •5.9 The tree branching of cultural traits.
- •5.10 Limitations of genetic and cultural co-evolutionary theory: Horizontal and vertical cultural evolution.
- •5.11 Cultural stability: Processes countering cultural evolution.
- •5.11.1 Migration and cultural stability.
- •5.11.2 Conformity and geographical mechanisms affecting cultural evolution and language development.
- •5.12 Social learning: Imitating success.
- •5.13 Religion, agriculture development and cultural evolution.
- •5.14 Phylogenetic evidence of the socio-cultural origins of language and other cultural traits.
- •5.14.1 Tracing the evolution of languages.
- •5.14.2 Evidence of language evolution.
- •5.15 Culture as a function of evolving information.
- •5.16 How did language evolve?
- •5.16.1 Contacts between different language speakers.
- •5.16.2 Artefactual languages.
- •Cognition: our common biology and cultural impact
- •6.1 Culture and cognition.
- •6.1.1 Sensation and perception.
- •6.1.2 Cultural impact on sensation and perception.
- •6.2 Cognitive development.
- •6.3 Cognitive style and cultural values.
- •6.3.1 Field dependent and independent cognitive style.
- •6.3.2 Perception studies and cognitive style.
- •6.3.3 Collectivistic and individualistic cognition.
- •6.3.4 Greek versus Asian thinking style.
- •6.3.5 Dialectical and logical thinking.
- •6.3.6 Authoritarianism and dogmatism as a cognitive style.
- •6.4 The general processor implied in cognitive styles versus contextualized cognition.
- •6.5 Cognitive style and priming cognition.
- •6.6 Cross-cultural differences in cognition as a function of practical imperatives.
- •6.7 Intelligence and adaptation: general and cross-cultural aspects.
- •6.7.1 Definitions of general intelligence.
- •6.7.2 Nature or nurture: What determines intelligence?
- •6.7.3 Sources of bias in intelligence testing.
- •6.7.4 Socioeconomic differences and fairness.
- •6.7.5 Race and the interaction effect.
- •6.8 The use of psychological tests in varying cultures.
- •6.9 How intelligence is viewed in other cultures.
- •6.10 General processes in higher order cognition and intelligence.
- •6.10.1 Categorization.
- •6.10.2 Memory functions.
- •6.10.3 Mathematical abilities.
- •6.10.4 The ultimate pedagogical goal: Creativity.
- •Summary
- •Emotions and human happiness: universal expressions and cultural values
- •7.1 The universality of emotions: Basic neurophysiological responses.
- •7.1.1 How we understand the emotion of others: Facial expressions.
- •7.1.2 The effect of language and learning: Criticisms of studies supporting genetically based facial recognition.
- •7.1.3 The definitive answer to the source of the facial expressions of emotions: Biology is the determinant.
- •7.1.4 Universal agreement and cultural emphasis in other emotion constructs.
- •7.1.4.1 Antecedents of emotions.
- •7.1.4.2 Vocalization and intonation in emotional expression.
- •7.1.4.3 Appraisal of emotion.
- •7.2 The role of culture in emotional reactions.
- •7.2.1 The display of emotions.
- •7.2.2 Individualistic versus collectivistic cultures: Display rules in emotion intensity and negativity ratings.
- •7.2.3 Personal space and gestures: Cultural influences in non-verbal communication.
- •7.2.4 Cross-cultural differences in evaluating emotions in other people.
- •7.3. The cultural context of emotional communication.
- •7.4 Toward a positive psychology of emotion: Happiness and well-being.
- •7.4.1 Methodological issues in definitions of happiness and well-being.
- •7.4.2 Sources of well-being.
- •7.4.3 The trending of happiness scores and economic crises and transitions.
- •7.4.4 The impact of culture on happiness and subjective well-being.
- •7.4.5 Creating social policies that promote well-being.
- •7.4.6 The role of national and local government.
- •Personality theory: western, eastern and indigenous approaches
- •8.1 Western thoughts on personality.
- •8.1.1 Freud’s contributions.
- •8.1.2 The humanistic approach to personality.
- •8.1.3 Social-cognitive interaction theory.
- •8.1.4 Locus of control
- •8.1.5 Cross-cultural research on locus of control and autonomy: In control or being controlled.
- •8.1.6 Personality types and hardwired foundations.
- •8.1.7 The Big Five.
- •8.1.8 The genetic and evolutionary basis of personality.
- •8.1.9 Is national character a psychological reality?
- •8.2 Eastern thoughts about personality.
- •8.2.1 The Buddhist tradition.
- •8.2.2 The self and causation.
- •8.2.3 Buddhism and consciousness.
- •8.2.4 Buddhism as a therapeutic approach.
- •8.2.5 A critical thought.
- •8.3 Confucian perspective on personality and the self.
- •8.4 Culture specific personality: As seen from the perspective of indigenous cultures.
- •8.5 Some evaluative comments on Confucianism and indigenous psychology.
- •Summary
- •Culture, sex and gender
- •10.1 Culture and gender.
- •10.1.1 Sex roles, gender stereotypes, and culture.
- •10.1. 2 Gender and families.
- •10.1.3 Traditional versus egalitarian sex role ideologies.
- •10.2 Gender stereotypes and discrimination against women.
- •10.2.1 Dissatisfaction with body image.
- •10.2.2 Equal work equal pay?
- •10.3 Violence against women: a dirty page of history and contemporary society.
- •10.3.1 Intimate violence: The ubiquitous nature of rape.
- •10.3.2 Sexual exploitation.
- •10.3.3 Gender justice and the empowerment of women.
- •10.3.4 Gender ability differences and the role of culture.
- •10.3.5 Culture and Gender differences in spatial abilities.
- •10.3.6 Current research on gender differences in mathematical abilities.
- •10.3.7 Gender and conformity.
- •10.3.8 Gender and aggression.
- •10.4 Sexual behavior and culture.
- •10.4.1 Mate selection.
- •10.4.2 Attractiveness and culture.
- •10.4.3 The future of love and marriage.
- •Summary
- •Culture and human health
- •12.1 The injustice of health disparities in the world.
- •12.1.1 Socio-economic disparities and well-being.
- •12.1.2 Mental health among ethnic minorities: Injustice in the United States.
- •12.1.3 Migrants, refugees and stress: Mental health outcomes.
- •12.2 The role of culture.
- •12.2.1 Cultural health beliefs.
- •12.2.2 Problems in cultural definitions of abnormality and mental illness
- •12.3 Psychopathology as universal or relativist.
- •12.4 Culturally specific and universal factors in mental health.
- •12.4.1 Anxiety disorders.
- •12.4.2 Regulation of mood: Depression.
- •12.4.3 Schizophrenia.
- •12.4.4 Attention deficit disorder.
- •12.4.5 Personality disorders.
- •12.5 Culturally sensitive assessment of abnormal behavior.
- •12.6 Cross-cultural assessments of mental disorder.
- •12.7 Abnormal behavior and psychotherapy from cultural perspectives.
- •12.7.1 The cultural framework matters in psychotherapy.
- •12.7.2 Homogeneity of patient and therapist.
- •12.7.3 Approaches based in indigenous forms of treatment.
- •12.7.4 Adding the biomedical model to indigenous beliefs.
- •Summary
3.5.1 The evolution of evolutionary theories.
As in other fields of social science modern sociocultural theories are careful to avoid ethnocentrism that is used to make value judgment about cultures and in cross-cultural comparisons. The modern perspective is to view societies within their own historical context. Cultural evolution found in neo-evolutionist theories are now considered multi-lineal as it is determined by many complex sources. Neo-evolutionists discard the deterministic arguments of the preceding century, and suggest that sociocultural evolution is a function of probability where accidental factors as well as the will of members of society play important roles. In place of value judgment the neo-evolutionists emphasize the importance of empirical data constituting measurable information that permits a scientific investigation of sociocultural evolution and associated processes.
Some modern theories (Wilson, 1975) sought to apply biological theory in social sciences by pointing to the evolutionary mechanisms that determines such varied social behaviors like aggression, nurturance and altruism. Sociobiologists argue that humans are the products of both biological evolution, but also analogous sociocultural evolution. Each evolutionary force is distinct and based on separate selective mechanisms and forms of transmission.
3.5.2 Dual inheritance: Approaches to cultural transmission.
The early discussion in this chapter has focused on the genetic architecture of cultural transmission. The challenge is to provide a space for both biological and cultural factors within a single theory. Explanation of cultural transmission requires a complex mathematical model given the complexity of each form of transmission and the interaction between biology and culture. Lumsden and Wilson (1981) argued that human cultural transmission should be understood within the framework of gene-culture transmission tracing human development from the genes to the human mind to resulting culture and is a product of gene-culture co-evolution. Rather than accepting that human beings pursue their interests based on biological needs expressed in many arbitrary culturally acquired behaviors, Lumsden and Wilson argued for the presence of epigenetic rules that provided greater specificity for the canalization of the mind.
Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman (1981) promoted a quantitative approach to sociocultural evolution since the lateral theory of biological evolution is primarily based on the mathematics of population genetics. Recognizing that it is at best difficult to partition transmission into genetic and cultural factors they sought to understand the dynamics of change by evaluating relative frequencies of cultural traits within given populations. Lumsden and Wilson advocated the concept of cultugens as the basic component of inheritance. The epigenetic rules that provided direction were genetically determined peripheral sensory filters including inter-neuron coding processes, but also cognitive parameters of perception, learning and decision-making. These factors together determined whether units of culture were transmitted or some substitute. From this perspective both genetic and cultural evolution work together as culture is shaped by biological processes and over the long run biological processes change in response to cultural alterations like when societies develop a more efficient food supply.
Boyd and Richerson (1985) advocated what is known as a dual inheritance system. Biological and sociocultural sources of transmission tend toward abstractness and complexity when researchers use a single psychological model. Boyd and Richerson advocated a dual inheritance model that in addition to genetic inheritance posited cultural transmission via the rules for social learning. Specifically they argued for a role of social learning in the transmission of cultural information. What is learned during the lifetime of an individual is obviously not transmitted genetically. However, the capacity for learning may have changed with new innovations like the use of computers. Whereas genetic alterations cannot be observed in a single lifetime, during such a limited time period cultural information can be passed on to the next generation. Cultural information responding to social needs may have consequences for an entire population as can be seen in the political transformation of much of the world. Experiences that are significant can be passed on to cultural offspring, and become part of the cultural heritage, whereas genetic transmission is only possible through differential rates of reproduction of significant traits.
The basic point of departure for Richerson and Wilson for their dual inheritance model is the analogy between genetic and cultural transmission. The relationship between genes and culture presented in their view the most interesting problem for science. The evolution of cultural transmissions in humans is analogous to the evolution of our genetic system. However, genes and culture are distinct systems of inheritance in humanity, and genetic predispositions and cultural predispositions result directly from distinct sources. Richerson and Boyd applied the mathematical standards of population genetics to model the adaptive and selective nature of culture. They argued that cultural evolution exists as a separate arena and is not mechanically related to genetic evolution. Most importantly, cultural evolution is more rapid, and ultimately more influential in social development. From their perspective cultural evolution can be understood by exactly the same processes as genetic evolution since selection for fitness is the ultimate determinant. Hinde (1982) on the other hand suggested that cultural and genetic evolution proceed independently. In summary in sociocultural theories genetics have not been overruled by the force of culture but the contribution of genes can best be understood as the foundation that enables cultural behavior and is manifested in the reciprocal relationship between genes and culture.
The major vehicle of cultural transmission is social learning as articulated by Bandura (1977). It would be too cumbersome a task for children to learn complex social behavior by means of conditioning, and imitation is seen as a much more economic model of cultural transmission. Learning by imitation explains the cultural stability we see in culturally defined behavior patterns. Boyd and Richerson see cultural transmission to be analogous to genetic transmission of information. Although all members of a culture may be exposed to similar culture individual exposure allows for personal variants. The relative variance of the cultural repository would depend on the static or fluid nature of the culture. Very static cultures produce few variants from which to select the most adaptive. Cultural conformity is an option of selection however, where the individual simply follows the behavior of the majority. In turn conformity reduces cultural variance in behavior within the cultural group while increasing variance between other cultural groups.
Evaluating the transmission of culture from these two perspectives seem simplistic considering the complexities of interaction. For example Laland, Odling-Smee, and Feldman (2000) argue that species interact with the environment and in the process create niches that modify the ecological context. Hinde’s (1987) theory emphasizes the importance of levels of social complexity and that at each level of social complexity culture has components not found at lower levels of analysis. Society, groups and individuals must all be considered interacting components in sociocultural evolution yet each with distinct properties. Genetic influences may not be direct and in any event genetic influence decreases in the context of culture. Campbell (1974,) promoted several levels of sociocultural transmission including genetic adaptation, learning and imitation, cultural accumulation, and the changes wrought by science. These influences are all governed by the evolutionary principles of selection of functional traits and the gradual elimination of non-adaptive components. Evidence from cross-cultural research demonstrates the complexity of cultural variation and the difficulty of tracing the interactions between genetic and learned components.
All human social groups have culture, and the essential issue addressed by this chapter is the evolutionary source of sociocultural change over time. Theories must address the origin of change and the transmission of the fundamental elements including the creation of varying social and economic systems, the development of science and religion, moral systems, and most basically the role of language. Similar to genetic evolution, natural selection played a primary role in sociocultural development as groups respond to what is adaptive in their particular environments. The theories discussed above seek to define the relationship between the genetic and cultural co-evolution. However, due to the complexity of interactions there is little empirical support for the formal mathematical models based on population genetics. We are really in the early stages of the formulation of theories that adequately represent the relationships between the two forces of evolution, although the same principles of selection and adaptation may be at play a role in both types. The role of conscious reflection and creativity are fundamental to social development, and likewise the use of symbols and language evolution. These issues are largely overlooked in sociocultural evolutionary theories, although there would be no origin of cultural systems without creative innovation and no transmission of human culture without language.